Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science Memory

Psychology study sheds light on why some moments seem to fly by

by Eric W. Dolan
May 24, 2025
in Memory
[Adobe Stock]

[Adobe Stock]

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Follow PsyPost on Google News

Many people feel that life speeds up as they get older. But what actually causes this sensation? A new study published in Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin sheds light on the reasons people perceive past periods of their lives as having passed quickly. Across four studies, researchers found that the feeling that time has flown by is more strongly associated with personal growth, satisfaction, and nostalgia than with daily routines or a lack of meaningful experiences.

Across cultures and age groups, people consistently report that time seems to pass faster as they grow older. This perception isn’t just a curiosity—it can influence life satisfaction, existential anxiety, and engagement with long-term goals. Despite its impact, the underlying psychological mechanisms remain poorly understood.

Historically, a common explanation is the routine-compression theory: when life is filled with repetitive tasks and few novel events, people form fewer unique memories. In retrospect, the time feels empty or collapsed, and thus shorter. An alternative hypothesis, based on self-determination theory, suggests that when people feel they did not experience personal growth, they judge the time as wasted and brief.

The researchers behind the new study sought to evaluate both theories and explore new possibilities. They wondered whether the perception that time flew by might actually be driven by how satisfied people felt with that period—and whether nostalgia could also play a role.

Across four studies involving nearly 2,500 participants in total, the researchers used surveys to examine how people recalled different time periods—such as the past year, a college semester, or the summer. They assessed participants’ memories of how routine or varied the period was, how much personal growth they felt they experienced, how many events stood out, and how satisfying or nostalgic the period felt.

In the first two studies, participants included university students reflecting on an academic year and a broader adult sample recruited online reflecting on their past year. In the final two studies, the team gathered responses from another online adult sample and from a new group of college students, this time reflecting on the past summer.

Participants responded to questions on a range of topics. These included how repetitive their days felt, how much they grew in terms of autonomy and competence, how many events they could recall, how satisfied they were with the period, and how nostalgic they felt about it. They also rated how fast that period felt in hindsight using slider scales.

The traditional routine-compression account received weak support. In two of the four studies, people who remembered a period as more routine also felt it had passed more quickly. But routine did not consistently predict how many events participants remembered. And even when it did, fewer events were not linked to slower time perception—in fact, sometimes the opposite was true.

The self-determination-based growth-deprivation hypothesis also did not hold up. Rather than feeling that time flew when they lacked growth, participants were more likely to say time sped by during periods when they did feel they were growing. This unexpected pattern led the researchers to reconsider their framework.

They proposed two new explanations. One was the growth-immersion account. According to this view, people feel that time passed quickly when they were deeply immersed in meaningful, challenging activities that supported their personal development. The satisfaction that came from those activities might have made them less aware of the passage of time—similar to how people lose track of time during a “flow” state.

The second was the growth-longing account. Here, the idea is that looking back on a time of growth may trigger a sense of nostalgic longing. The period might feel special and emotionally significant, but also fleeting, because it stands out as a high point in one’s personal development.

To test these new ideas, the researchers looked at whether feelings of satisfaction and nostalgia explained the link between remembered growth and time perception. Across two pre-registered studies, they found that both satisfaction and nostalgia predicted faster perceived time passage. And when these factors were included in statistical models, the direct effect of growth on perceived speed disappeared. This suggests that growth may influence time perception indirectly, by increasing satisfaction and nostalgia.

Between the two new explanations, satisfaction appeared to be a slightly stronger predictor of how fast time seemed to pass. Still, both played significant roles.

The studies relied on self-reports and cross-sectional data, which means they can’t definitively establish causation. It’s also possible that people who generally feel more nostalgic or satisfied with life are more likely to perceive time as passing quickly, regardless of actual events. Future research could use longitudinal or experimental methods to better understand how growth, satisfaction, and nostalgia shape time perception.

Age and lifestyle may also moderate how routine or growth is experienced. The routine-compression effect was more evident in older, more demographically diverse samples than in younger college students. This suggests that the meaning of “routine” may shift with age or life circumstances.

Finally, the researchers noted that people might be remembering different types of events when asked to recall memorable experiences. Their studies did not distinguish between ordinary daily events and major life milestones, which might have different effects on time perception.

The feeling that life flies by may not be a symptom of monotony or wasted time, as some theories suggest. Instead, it may reflect the opposite: periods filled with growth, satisfaction, and rich emotional meaning tend to feel shorter in hindsight. Rather than worrying that time is slipping away too fast, people might reframe that experience as a sign that the period was meaningful and well-lived.

This shift in perspective could help individuals come to terms with the pace of life and may even encourage them to seek out more self-determined, fulfilling experiences. As the authors suggest, perhaps the goal shouldn’t be to slow life down—but to make it rich enough that its swift passage feels worthwhile.

“Time appeared to pass swiftly when it followed a repetitive pattern of routine, but critically, when it also was fulfilling, evoking a sense of satisfaction and immersion, and perhaps to a lesser extent, nostalgia and longing,” the researchers concluded. “The growth-immersion and growth-longing mechanisms converge to suggest that time flies because you got a lot out of it. This conclusion points to a novel intervention to encourage people to reappraise life’s apparent acceleration as a sign of a meaningful life. In other words, perhaps the goal should not be to ‘slow life down’ at all, but rather to encourage people to interpret the feeling that a period zipped by as a sign that that period was well lived. Life goes fast, we say, but you wouldn’t want it any other way.”

The study, “Why Life Moves Fast: Exploring the Mechanisms Behind Autobiographical Time Perception,” was authored by Young-Ju Ryu, Mark J. Landau, Samuel E. Arnold, and Jamie Arndt.

RELATED

New research sheds light on cannabinoids’ impact on anxiety during alcohol withdrawal
Cannabis

Surprising study suggests cannabis can improve some types of memory in the aging brain

July 22, 2025

Does cannabis help or harm memory in older age? A new study in rats suggests the answer is complex. Researchers found THC’s effects depended on sex and delivery method, improving working memory in some cases while impairing it in others.

Read moreDetails
Even in healthy adults, high blood sugar levels are linked to impaired brain function
Memory

Neuroscientists decode how people juggle multiple items in working memory

July 8, 2025

New neuroscience research shows how the brain decides which memories deserve more attention. By tracking brain activity, scientists found that the frontal cortex helps direct limited memory resources, allowing people to remember high-priority information more precisely than less relevant details.

Read moreDetails
From fireflies to brain cells: Unraveling the complex web of synchrony in networks
Addiction

Understanding “neuronal ensembles” could revolutionize addiction treatment

July 3, 2025

The same brain system that rewards you for a delicious meal is hijacked by drugs like fentanyl. A behavioral neuroscientist explains how understanding the specific memories behind these rewards is the key to treating addiction without harming our essential survival instincts.

Read moreDetails
Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time
Memory

Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time

July 3, 2025

Our perception of time is more fragile than we think. Scientists have uncovered a powerful illusion where repeated exposure to information makes us misremember it as happening much further in the past, significantly distorting our mental timelines.

Read moreDetails
Researchers identify neural mechanism behind memory prioritization
Memory

Researchers identify neural mechanism behind memory prioritization

June 30, 2025

A new brain imaging study shows that when people try to remember multiple things, their brains give more precise attention to the most important item. The frontal cortex helps allocate memory resources, boosting accuracy for high-priority information.

Read moreDetails
Muscle contractions release chemical signals that promote brain network development
Memory

Sleep helps stitch memories into cognitive maps, according to new neuroscience breakthrough

June 28, 2025

Scientists have discovered that forming a mental map of a new environment takes more than just recognizing individual places—it also requires sleep. The study highlights how weakly tuned neurons gradually become synchronized to encode space as a connected whole.

Read moreDetails
The fading affect bias impacts most memories — but election-related memories are surprisingly resilient
Memory

Scientists shed light on how forgiveness does and doesn’t reshape memories

June 20, 2025

A new study suggests that forgiving someone does not make us forget what they did—but it does change how we feel about it. People who forgave recalled past wrongs with just as much detail, but with less emotional pain.

Read moreDetails
Poor sleep may shrink brain regions vulnerable to Alzheimer’s disease, study suggests
Memory

Neuroscientists discover biological mechanism that helps the brain ignore irrelevant information

June 14, 2025

New research suggests the brain uses a learning rule at inhibitory synapses to block out distractions during memory replay. This process enables the hippocampus to prioritize useful patterns over random noise, helping build more generalizable and reliable memories.

Read moreDetails

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Microplastics found to obstruct brain blood vessels in troubling neuroscience study

Scientists map the visual patterns people use when evaluating others’ bodies

The ADHD symptom no one talks about: rejection sensitive dysphoria

How stress affects us may depend on the balance between testosterone and cortisol

Women who fake orgasm tend to struggle with emotional clarity and impulse control, study finds

What we know about a person changes how our brain processes their face

Dementia diagnoses are often delayed for years, new study finds

Are “zombie cells” in your blood vessels driving long-COVID and chronic fatigue?

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy