Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science Memory

Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time

by Eric W. Dolan
July 3, 2025
in Memory
Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay informed on the latest psychology and neuroscience research—follow PsyPost on LinkedIn for daily updates and insights.

When something is repeated often, we tend to remember it better. But a new study published in Psychological Science has found that we may also mistakenly believe that repeated experiences happened longer ago than they actually did. Across six experiments involving hundreds of adults, researchers found that when an image or piece of information was presented multiple times, people consistently judged its first appearance as being further in the past than it really was. This illusion of time—called the “temporal repetition effect”—was surprisingly strong, distorting people’s memory of when something happened by as much as 25 percent.

This finding challenges a long-held assumption in psychology: that the vividness or strength of a memory helps us gauge how recently something happened. According to this idea, if a memory feels faint or unclear, we assume it happened a long time ago. If it feels fresh and clear, we assume it was more recent. But the results of this study suggest that things we remember well—precisely because they were repeated—might actually seem older in our memory.

The researchers were inspired by everyday experiences. “We all know what it is like to be bombarded with the same headline day after day after day. We wondered whether this constant repetition of information was distorting our mental timelines,” said study author Sami Yousif, who will be an assistant professors at The Ohio State University after August 1.

Even if that headline first appeared just a few days ago, it might begin to feel like it happened weeks ago simply because of how often it has been repeated. This intuition led the researchers to ask whether repetition itself could create a distortion in how far back in time we think something happened.

To explore this idea, Yousif and his co-author — Brynn E. Sherman — conducted a series of experiments using object images and online memory tasks. In the first experiment, 50 participants viewed a sequence of images of everyday objects, such as a stapler or a watering can. Some images were shown only once, while others were repeated across five blocks of trials. After viewing the sequence, participants were shown the same images again and asked to indicate when in the experiment they had first seen each one. They did this by moving a marker along a visual timeline.

The key design of the experiment allowed for a direct comparison: each repeated image was paired with a non-repeated image that had been shown right before it in the original sequence. This meant that both images were originally seen at nearly the same time. But despite this, participants consistently remembered the repeated images as having occurred earlier. This illusion remained strong even when participants successfully recognized the images, suggesting that memory accuracy for the items themselves wasn’t driving the effect.

The researchers also found that the more times an image was repeated, the stronger the illusion became. Images shown five times were remembered as having occurred even further back than those shown only two or three times. This pattern persisted across all seven sets of image conditions.

“We were surprised at how strong the effects were,” Yousif told PsyPost. “We had a hunch that repetition might distort temporal memory, but we did not expect these distortions to be so significant.”

To test whether this illusion was due to memory strategies or shortcuts—such as participants assuming that anything repeated must have come earlier—the researchers ran a follow-up version of the experiment with added survey questions. Participants were asked to describe their strategies and to estimate how often they relied on repetition when placing images in time. Although some participants reported using such strategies, these responses did not predict the strength of the illusion. That means the effect likely wasn’t the result of deliberate reasoning, but rather something deeper in how memory and time perception interact.

Another experiment ruled out the possibility that pauses between blocks of images, which might signal shifts in context or time, were responsible for the effect. When participants viewed the same kinds of repeated images without any breaks between blocks, the illusion remained just as strong.

A third experiment tested whether the illusion would still appear if people were told ahead of time that they would be asked to judge the timing of each image’s first appearance. Even when participants were explicitly warned to focus on when they saw an image for the first time, the repetition still caused them to misjudge when that first exposure occurred.

In a fourth experiment, the researchers had participants indicate during the viewing phase whether each image was “new” or “old” each time it appeared. This allowed the team to track which repetitions participants actually remembered. Again, the illusion held: the more times people remembered seeing an image, the more likely they were to place its first appearance earlier in time. A statistical analysis showed that it wasn’t just the number of times an image was shown, but the number of times it was remembered that mattered most.

To strengthen their results, the researchers changed the format in a fifth experiment. Instead of using a timeline, they showed participants two images—one repeated, one not—and asked which one they saw first. Even though the non-repeated image had always come first, participants chose the repeated image as having come first nearly 80 percent of the time.

Finally, in a sixth experiment, the team tested whether this effect held up over a longer period. Participants viewed image sequences across five days and returned three days later to complete the memory test. Even with this extended timeline, people continued to remember repeated images as having occurred earlier. This finding suggests that the illusion isn’t just a short-term quirk, but a lasting feature of memory that may shape how we recall events across days or even weeks.

This new research sheds light on a puzzling phenomenon: our sense of time doesn’t always match the calendar. The feeling that something happened “ages ago” may not be a reliable indicator of when it truly occurred, especially if that something was repeated many times. The study also highlights a gap in current memory theories. Many psychological models assume that stronger memories are perceived as more recent, but these results show the opposite.

“People should take away two things: (1) Time perception is illusory. That is, our sense of when things occurred is systematically distorted in predictable ways. (2) These distortions can be substantial, even if their causes are simple (i.e., the mere repetition of information),” Yousif said.

There are several possible reasons why this illusion happens, according to the researchers. One idea is that when an item is repeated, later presentations may “remind” us of the earlier ones, effectively reinforcing the memory of the first appearance and embedding it more deeply. Over time, this could make the initial event feel like it happened further in the past. Another idea is that our sense of when something happened is not stored directly in memory, but is instead reconstructed using cues such as repetition. In that case, repetition could trick us into thinking an event happened longer ago than it really did.

As with all research, there are some limitations. All the participants were English-speaking adults living in the United States, so the results may not apply to all cultures or age groups. And although the experiments were designed to mimic real-world memory experiences, they still took place in a controlled digital environment using images of objects, not complex or emotionally charged events.

Still, the researchers emphasized the consistency of the findings across different experimental designs. “We tested the phenomenon in a variety of different ways, and it was incredibly robust in all of those cases,” Yousif said. “This strikes us as a particularly strong effect.”

The researchers hope that future work will explore how this illusion affects different types of memories—like emotional events, social interactions, or news stories—and how it might influence decision-making, planning, and even public opinion. They also suggest studying whether the effect operates differently in children, older adults, or people with memory impairments.

“We remain interested in time perception at the scale of ordinary experience,” Yousif explained. “In other words, we really want to know what other factors influence how our mental timelines are shaped by our experience.”

The study, “An Illusion of Time Caused by Repeated Experience,” was published online on April 18, 2025.

TweetSendScanShareSendPin1ShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

From fireflies to brain cells: Unraveling the complex web of synchrony in networks
Addiction

Understanding “neuronal ensembles” could revolutionize addiction treatment

July 3, 2025

The same brain system that rewards you for a delicious meal is hijacked by drugs like fentanyl. A behavioral neuroscientist explains how understanding the specific memories behind these rewards is the key to treating addiction without harming our essential survival instincts.

Read moreDetails
Researchers identify neural mechanism behind memory prioritization
Memory

Researchers identify neural mechanism behind memory prioritization

June 30, 2025

A new brain imaging study shows that when people try to remember multiple things, their brains give more precise attention to the most important item. The frontal cortex helps allocate memory resources, boosting accuracy for high-priority information.

Read moreDetails
Muscle contractions release chemical signals that promote brain network development
Memory

Sleep helps stitch memories into cognitive maps, according to new neuroscience breakthrough

June 28, 2025

Scientists have discovered that forming a mental map of a new environment takes more than just recognizing individual places—it also requires sleep. The study highlights how weakly tuned neurons gradually become synchronized to encode space as a connected whole.

Read moreDetails
The fading affect bias impacts most memories — but election-related memories are surprisingly resilient
Memory

Scientists shed light on how forgiveness does and doesn’t reshape memories

June 20, 2025

A new study suggests that forgiving someone does not make us forget what they did—but it does change how we feel about it. People who forgave recalled past wrongs with just as much detail, but with less emotional pain.

Read moreDetails
Poor sleep may shrink brain regions vulnerable to Alzheimer’s disease, study suggests
Memory

Neuroscientists discover biological mechanism that helps the brain ignore irrelevant information

June 14, 2025

New research suggests the brain uses a learning rule at inhibitory synapses to block out distractions during memory replay. This process enables the hippocampus to prioritize useful patterns over random noise, helping build more generalizable and reliable memories.

Read moreDetails
Democrats dislike Republicans more than Republicans dislike Democrats, studies find
Memory

Reduced memory specificity linked to earlier onset of psychiatric disorders in youth

June 11, 2025

New research suggests that difficulty recalling specific personal memories may be an early warning sign of mental illness in youth. A meta-analysis finds this memory trait predicts first-time psychiatric diagnoses, especially depression, during adolescence and early adulthood.

Read moreDetails
Neuroscientists pinpoint part of the brain that deciphers memory from new experience
Memory

Neuroscientists find individual differences in memory response to amygdala stimulation

May 31, 2025

Stimulating the brain’s amygdala during memory formation can boost recall after 24 hours, a new study finds. But the effect varies: some people’s memory improves, others’ worsens—and baseline memory performance appears to be the best predictor of outcome.

Read moreDetails
Psychology study sheds light on why some moments seem to fly by
Memory

Psychology study sheds light on why some moments seem to fly by

May 24, 2025

A new study suggests life feels like it speeds up during periods of personal growth and satisfaction. Rather than routine making time seem short, researchers found that fulfilled, nostalgic memories are more likely to make the past feel like a blur.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Many ayahuasca users report challenging experiences—yet some are linked to better mental health

These common sounds can impair your learning, according to new psychology research

Hispanic adolescents experience later sleep timing and greater social jet lag than peers, study finds

Student loan debt doesn’t deter civic engagement — it may actually drive it, new research suggests

Understanding “neuronal ensembles” could revolutionize addiction treatment

Not bothered by celebrity infidelity? This psychological trait might be why

Genetic factors may influence how well exercise buffers against childhood trauma

Tips for parents in talking with your kids about your partner’s mental illness

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy