Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology

New psychology research: Privilege has a smaller influence on positive world beliefs than you think

by Eric W. Dolan
October 12, 2023
in Social Psychology
(Photo credit: Adobe Firefly)

(Photo credit: Adobe Firefly)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay informed on the latest psychology and neuroscience research—follow PsyPost on LinkedIn for daily updates and insights.

Contrary to popular intuition, a recent scientific study has found that our beliefs about the world are not strongly tied to our personal life experiences. The study, published in the Journal of Personality, defies the common assumption that people from privileged backgrounds are more likely to have a positive outlook on life. Instead, the research suggests that personal experiences may have a less significant influence on our beliefs about the world than previously thought.

The motivation behind this study was to explore and better understand the relationship between an individual’s life experiences and their primal world beliefs, a concept in psychology that refers to the fundamental beliefs or assumptions that individuals hold about the nature of the world.

“This project was the idea of my colleague Jer Clifton, who is co-author on this paper,” said study co-author Nicholas Kerry, a postdoctoral fellow at the University of Pennsylvania’s Positive Psychology Center. “While running numerous studies on people’s beliefs about the world, he noticed that many people seem to attribute their own beliefs to their upbringing (e.g. ‘I see the world as a dangerous place because I grew up poor’). ”

“Similarly, people who expressed positive beliefs on social media (e.g. ‘the world is a good place’) were sometimes told that their beliefs must be the result of their privileged circumstances or experiences. But it was unclear whether these were isolated anecdotes or commonly held assumptions, and also whether they correspond to reality.”

“Accordingly, we designed several studies to test a) whether laypeople and researchers really do have the intuition that beliefs are strongly related to experiences and circumstances, and b) whether this intuition is correct. We found that many people—both laypeople and researchers—do seem to expect big relationships between personal experiences and beliefs about the world, but we found that these relationships are actually much smaller than people expect.”

In the first study, the researchers surveyed 494 laypeople and 486 researchers to gauge their expectations regarding the relationship between privilege and world beliefs. This step helped establish the common assumptions that people have about how personal experiences shape our views of the world.

In the second study, the researchers analyzed data from a massive sample of 14,481 individuals. They explored various factors associated with privilege, including gender, wealth, health, and living conditions, and their correlation with primal world beliefs. Importantly, they compared these correlations with the expectations expressed by the surveyed participants in the first study.

The third study focused on the impact of traumatic experiences on world beliefs. This study included 1,086 participants, including cancer survivors, people living with cystic fibrosis, and those who had caused accidents resulting in death or injury. The researchers sought to understand how individuals who had faced such traumatic events perceived the world.

The researchers found that the commonly held belief that privilege leads to more positive worldviews is not strongly supported by the data. Contrary to expectations, there were weak correlations between indicators of privilege (being male, being wealthy, avoiding health issues, etc.) and positive primal world beliefs.

“I think this study suggests that the way we see the world is not as closely rooted in our circumstances as a lot of people think,” Kerry told PsyPost. “It may remind us not to make assumptions: knowing that someone sees things positively does not mean that they have not experienced adversity.”

The strongest relationship found in the study was between experiencing childhood trauma and the belief that the world is a dangerous place. Individuals who had faced trauma tended to have more negative worldviews, aligning with “Shattered Assumptions Theory,” which suggests that traumatic events can shatter positive world assumptions and lead to mental health issues like depression.

The study also found that individuals living with health conditions like cystic fibrosis or cancer did not necessarily view the world as worse or more unjust than others. Even current cancer patients had only slightly more negative worldviews than healthy controls.

“I think it was especially interesting that serious illness wasn’t associated with substantial differences in worldviews,” Kerry said. “For example, people with cystic fibrosis saw the world just as positively as a control group. And while people who currently had cancer saw the world as slightly less good and safe than controls, the difference was modest and this difference was not present among cancer survivors, suggesting that people’s beliefs are pretty resilient in the long-term.”

The study suggests that personal experiences might have less influence on world beliefs than commonly thought. However, this does not mean that life experiences have no impact whatsoever on one’s beliefs about the world.

“This is correlational work that examines averages across large groups of people,” Kerry explained. “It is possible that, for some people, experiences may have profound influences on the way they see the world. Our research simply suggests that the experiences we studied here do not routinely have large effects on most people.”

The study, “Despite popular intuition, positive world beliefs poorly reflect several objective indicators of privilege, including wealth, health, sex, and neighborhood safety“, was authored by Nicholas Kerry, KC White, Mark L. O’Brien, Laura M. Perry, and Jeremy D. W. Clifton.

TweetSendScanShareSendPin1ShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time
Racism and Discrimination

Hispanic adolescents experience later sleep timing and greater social jet lag than peers, study finds

July 3, 2025

Hispanic adolescents reported later sleep timing, greater social jet lag, shorter weekday sleep, higher caffeine intake, and more evening screen time than peers. Screen use and stress, but not caffeine, were linked to delayed sleep and sleep misalignment.

Read moreDetails
Racial and religious differences help explain why unmarried voters lean Democrat
Political Psychology

Student loan debt doesn’t deter civic engagement — it may actually drive it, new research suggests

July 3, 2025

Americans with student loan debt are more likely to vote and engage in political activities than those without debt, likely because they see government as responsible and capable of addressing their financial burden through policy change.

Read moreDetails
Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time
Infidelity

Not bothered by celebrity infidelity? This psychological trait might be why

July 3, 2025

The online shaming of unfaithful celebrities is a modern spectacle, but why do some join in while others don't? Researchers exploring this puzzle found a key predictor: a belief in a just world, which unexpectedly dampens outrage and blame.

Read moreDetails
Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time
Mental Health

New research suggests the conservative mental health advantage is a myth

July 3, 2025

Do conservatives really have better mental well-being than liberals? A new study suggests the answer depends entirely on how you ask. The well-known ideological gap disappears when "mental health" is replaced with the less-stigmatized phrase "overall mood."

Read moreDetails
New psychology study sheds light on mysterious “feelings of presence” during isolation
Political Psychology

People who think “everyone agrees with me” are more likely to support populism

July 1, 2025

People who wrongly believe that most others share their political views are more likely to support populist ideas, according to a new study. These false beliefs can erode trust in democratic institutions and fuel resentment toward political elites.

Read moreDetails
Researchers identify neural mechanism behind memory prioritization
Social Psychology

People who are more likely to die seem to care less about the future

June 30, 2025

Individuals with higher mortality risk—as judged by actuarial life insurance data—tend to care less about long-term consequences. They show more impulsivity and less future planning, consistent with evolutionary theories that link time horizon to environmental and internal health cues.

Read moreDetails
Scientists show how you’re unknowingly sealing yourself in an information bubble
Cognitive Science

Scientists show how you’re unknowingly sealing yourself in an information bubble

June 29, 2025

Scientists have found that belief polarization doesn’t always come from misinformation or social media bubbles. Instead, it often begins with a simple search. Our choice of words—and the algorithm’s response—can subtly seal us inside our own informational comfort zones.

Read moreDetails
Radical leaders inspire stronger devotion because they make followers feel significant, study finds
Political Psychology

Radical leaders inspire stronger devotion because they make followers feel significant, study finds

June 28, 2025

A new study finds that voters are more motivated by radical political leaders than moderates, because supporting bold causes makes them feel personally significant—driving greater activism, sacrifice, and long-term engagement across elections in the United States and Poland.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Hispanic adolescents experience later sleep timing and greater social jet lag than peers, study finds

Student loan debt doesn’t deter civic engagement — it may actually drive it, new research suggests

Understanding “neuronal ensembles” could revolutionize addiction treatment

Not bothered by celebrity infidelity? This psychological trait might be why

Genetic factors may influence how well exercise buffers against childhood trauma

Tips for parents in talking with your kids about your partner’s mental illness

Subjective cognitive struggles strongly linked to social recovery in depression

New research suggests the conservative mental health advantage is a myth

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy