Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

Study highlights the importance of napping for memory consolidation in early childhood

by Eric W. Dolan
May 28, 2023
in Cognitive Science
Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay informed on the latest psychology and neuroscience research—follow PsyPost on LinkedIn for daily updates and insights.

New research suggests that napping is beneficial for memory processing in early childhood. The study, published in Child Development, found that the combination of napping and overnight sleep improves retrieval of emotional memories compared to overnight sleep alone. The findings highlight the importance of naps in supporting memory and emotional processing in young children.

The study sought to investigate the impact of napping on emotional memory consolidation in early childhood. Specifically, the researchers wanted to determine if naps protect emotional memories from interference, indicating consolidation, or if napping only prepares memories for consolidation during overnight sleep. The study focused on emotional memories in the domain of social learning.

“We were led to this question from two directions. First, our work on naps in children showed us that naps at this age (preschool age, 3-5 yrs) benefit learning. Second, our work in adults has shown us that sleep is really important for emotional memory processing. So this led us to consider whether the naps in children benefit emotional processing,” explained study author Rebecca Spencer, a professor of psychological and brain sciences at the University of Massachusetts, Amherst.

The study involved 63 participants, aged 33 to 67 months, enrolled in preschools in western Massachusetts. Eligible children had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and no reported developmental, learning, sleep, or neurological disorders.

The participants were divided into two groups: an interference group and a no-interference group. The groups were matched in terms of age, sex, household income, and race/ethnicity. There were no significant differences in nap length or nap habituality between the groups. Children’s self-reported sleepiness and mood were also similar across the nap and wake conditions.

The task used in the study involved an emotional memory task adapted from previous research. During the encoding phase, neutral face images were paired with audio recordings presented through headphones. The audio consisted of neutral or mean descriptions referencing the faces, aiming to associate them with age-appropriate neutral or rule-violating actions.

The study included three recognition phases: immediate, delayed, and 24-hour recognition. Immediate recognition occurred right after the encoding phase, delayed recognition took place about 30 minutes after the child’s nap or equivalent wake period, and 24-hour recognition occurred the following morning, approximately 24 hours after the encoding phase.

In each recognition phase, children were presented with a simple memory task. They had to select the familiar face from a pair, which included one face from the encoding phase and one novel distractor face of the same gender.

The procedure involved two testing conditions: nap promotion and wake promotion. Each child participated in both conditions, with the order counterbalanced across participants. The encoding phase and immediate recognition phase were completed in the morning. Afterward, children returned to their normal classroom routine until the nap opportunity, during which they were either promoted to nap or to stay awake.

The interference group underwent the interference phase after their nap or wake intervals, followed by delayed recognition. During the interference phase, participants were exposed to stimuli that were unrelated to the emotional faces they had initially encountered. This was designed to create interference between the initial encoding of the emotional faces and the subsequent memory retrieval.

Spencer and her colleagues found that napping (without interference) provided immediate and next-day benefits to children’s emotional memory performance. In the interference group, napping showed mixed results depending on the emotional valence of the faces, while in the no-interference group, napping consistently improved memory accuracy over the 24-hour period.

Simply put, participants who napped the previous day showed improved face recognition. This suggests that the combination of napping and overnight sleep enhances the retrieval of emotional memories compared to overnight sleep alone.

“This study shows us that naps are important at this age,” Spencer told PsyPost. “They support memory and emotional processing and these functions can’t be made up for by overnight sleep – most kids need both naps and overnight sleep and when we withhold these, their memory and emotional reactivity can be compromised.”

“So until a child is ready to transition out, we should not push them. This is particularly important because with the move to universal pre-K, these preschools are not always allowing for a nap opportunity which could be counter-effective to the benefits of preschool.”

Surprisingly, the researchers observed a marginal memory decay effect for negatively valenced faces after napping. Although naps initially seemed to destabilize emotional memories in the short term, previous research has shown that this destabilization can lead to longer-term memory stability and enhanced consolidation.

“Initially we thought naps would give an immediate memory benefit but here we show for an emotional memory, right after the nap the memory gets worse, it is destabilized by sleep,” Spencer explained. “However, these are the memories that then do the best following subsequent overnight sleep so they still ‘win’ in the end. It just suggests a different mechanism as to how the memory evolves.”

The study’s findings provide valuable insights into how naps and overnight sleep interact during the processing of emotional memories. However, it is unclear whether naps are equally beneficial for emotional learning in children who are transitioning away from napping and those who nap regularly. Due to a small number of non-habitual nappers in the study, further analyses were not possible.

“It’s important to consider how these effects change as kids transition out of naps,” Spencer said. “As these were almost all habitual nappers, we could not discern how this changes as they are developmentally read to transition out of naps.”

The study, “Early childhood naps initiate emotional memory processing in preparation for enhanced overnight consolidation“, was authored by Olivia Hanron, Gina M. Mason, Jennifer F. Holmes, and Rebecca M. C. Spencer.

TweetSendScanShareSendPin7ShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Scientists use brain activity to predict StarCraft II skill in fascinating new neuroscience research
Cognitive Science

Scientists use brain activity to predict StarCraft II skill in fascinating new neuroscience research

May 16, 2025

A study combining brain scans and gameplay data reveals that players with more efficient visual attention and stronger white matter connections excel at StarCraft II. The results highlight how neural traits shape success in cognitively demanding video games.

Read moreDetails
Neuroscientists discover music’s hidden power to reshape memory
Memory

Neuroscientists discover music’s hidden power to reshape memory

May 14, 2025

A new neuroimaging study reveals that listening to emotionally charged music during memory recall can change how we remember events. The music not only shaped what participants remembered but also altered the emotional tone of their memories one day later.

Read moreDetails
Study links anomalous experiences to subconscious connectedness and other psychological traits
Cognitive Science

Study links anomalous experiences to subconscious connectedness and other psychological traits

May 13, 2025

A new study suggests that unusual experiences like déjà vu or premonitions are not only common but linked to a distinct psychological trait called subconscious connectedness. Researchers found that people high in this trait reported significantly more anomalous experiences.

Read moreDetails
Eye-tracking study suggests that negative comments on social media are more attention-grabbing than positive comments
Cognitive Science

Can you train your brain to unsee optical illusions? Scientists think so

May 12, 2025

A recent study found that radiologists are less susceptible to optical illusions, likely due to their intensive visual training. The research challenges long-standing beliefs that illusions are automatic and suggests perceptual skills can be shaped over time.

Read moreDetails
Diets high in fat and sugar appear to harm cognitive function
Cognitive Science

Diets high in fat and sugar appear to harm cognitive function

May 10, 2025

Consuming a Western-style diet packed with sugar and saturated fats may hurt your brain, not just your waistline. A new study shows poorer performance on spatial memory tasks among people with diets high in processed, unhealthy foods.

Read moreDetails
People with lower cognitive ability more likely to fall for pseudo-profound bullshit
Cognitive Science

People with lower cognitive ability more likely to fall for pseudo-profound bullshit

May 9, 2025

A new meta-analysis published in Applied Cognitive Psychology finds that people with lower cognitive ability are more likely to find meaning in pseudo-profound nonsense. The study identifies key psychological traits linked to susceptibility to feel-good but meaningless statements.

Read moreDetails
Neuroscientists uncover a fascinating fact about social thinking in the brain
Cognitive Science

Neuroscientists uncover a fascinating fact about social thinking in the brain

May 7, 2025

Our brains process social similarity in two ways—by comparing people to each other and by comparing them to ourselves. A new study using brain imaging reveals that these forms of person knowledge are represented in separate areas of the brain.

Read moreDetails
Classical music may promote calmer, more stable fetal heart rhythms, study suggests
Cognitive Science

Classical music may promote calmer, more stable fetal heart rhythms, study suggests

May 6, 2025

Listening to classical music may calm the fetal heartbeat, according to new research. The study found that heart rate patterns became more orderly after music exposure.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

From fixed pulses to smart stimulation: Parkinson’s treatment takes a leap forward

New research challenges idea that female breasts are sexualized due to modesty norms

Mother’s childhood trauma linked to emotional and behavioral issues in her children, study finds

New study sheds light on which post-psychedelic difficulties last longest and what helps people cope

Young adults who drink heavily report more romantic highs and lows

Amphetamine scrambles the brain’s sense of time by degrading prefrontal neuron coordination

New eye test may detect Alzheimer’s disease years before symptoms emerge, study suggests

Glymphatic dysfunction linked to cognitive performance deficits in adults with ADHD, study finds

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy