Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology Political Psychology

The confrontation effect revealed: Outrage over opposing political views fuels social media engagement

by Eric W. Dolan
October 14, 2024
in Political Psychology, Social Media
(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Follow PsyPost on Google News

Social media users are more likely to engage with posts that provoke rather than affirm their political beliefs, according to new research published in Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. This “confrontation effect” is driven by outrage, pushing people to respond to opposing viewpoints. The findings shed light on the emotional dynamics that drive online engagement.

The researchers — Daniel Mochon, the Edward H. Austin Jr. Professor of Business Administration and an associate professor of marketing at Tulane University’s A. B. Freeman School of Business, and Janet Schwartz, the executive director of Duke University’s Center for Advanced Hindsight — were motivated by a puzzling contradiction in existing knowledge about how people process information. Prior studies have shown that individuals tend to avoid content that challenges their beliefs, a behavior known as “confirmation bias.”

However, social media platforms frequently showcase highly polarized and combative interactions between individuals with opposing views. This raises questions about why people, who are generally expected to avoid conflicting information, engage more frequently with it online. Mochon and Schwartz sought to explain this paradox, hypothesizing that the outrage provoked by opposing views might be a key factor driving this unexpected engagement.

“We wanted to better understand how people interact with ideologically charged content online,” Mochon told PsyPost. “We commonly observed online settings where negative sentiments posted by users with ideology-inconsistent views dominated the threads, and we wanted to reconcile this pattern with the well-established finding that people tend to avoid information inconsistent with their beliefs.”

To investigate this, the researchers conducted a series of studies using a combination of real-world data from social media platforms and controlled online experiments.

In three initial field studies, the researchers aimed to test whether people engage more with content that opposes their political views (ideology-inconsistent) rather than content that aligns with their beliefs (ideology-consistent). They used Facebook’s advertising platform to target U.S. users with different political views (liberal and conservative).

The researchers created political posts related to three topics: gun control (Study 1A), Obamacare (Study 1B), and President Trump (Study 1C). Each post was designed to support either a liberal or conservative stance. For example, one post might advocate for gun control, while another opposes it. These posts appeared in users’ newsfeeds as paid advertisements.

The results showed that people were more likely to click on and comment on posts that opposed their political views than those that supported them. For example, liberals were more likely to engage with posts supporting President Trump than those criticizing him. This pattern, observed across all three studies, provided evidence for the confrontation effect.

“We were surprised by the size of the confrontation effect in the field studies,” Mochon said. “We found situations where users were four times more likely to engage with content they disagreed with than content they agreed with.”

Next, Mochon and Schwartz sought to replicate the findings of the Facebook field studies in a more controlled setting, allowing for a clearer understanding of the confrontation effect. The researchers also extended their investigation beyond politics to another emotionally charged issue: vegetarianism.

The study recruited 1,001 participants from Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk), an online platform commonly used for academic research. Participants were categorized based on their dietary practices (vegetarian/vegan or non-vegetarian) and were shown two tweets: one supporting vegetarianism and another opposing it. They were then given the option to comment on each tweet, and the researchers recorded whether participants chose to engage.

The findings mirrored those of the Facebook studies, with participants more likely to comment on tweets that conflicted with their dietary practices. For instance, non-vegetarians were more likely to respond to pro-vegetarian tweets, often expressing negative emotions such as anger or disgust.

The researchers then explored whether the type of engagement response moderated the confrontation effect. They hypothesized that when people are asked to comment on ideology-inconsistent content, they are more likely to engage than when asked if they want to follow or see more content from the same source.

Participants (802 individuals recruited from the research platform Prolific) were assigned to one of two conditions: they were either asked to comment on an ideology-inconsistent tweet or decide whether they wanted to see more content from the same source. The tweets addressed a highly charged topic: COVID-19 vaccines. Participants’ emotional reactions were measured, and the researchers recorded their engagement behavior in both conditions.

The results showed a stark difference depending on the type of engagement. Participants were more likely to comment on tweets that contradicted their views on vaccines (in line with the confrontation effect). However, when given the option to follow the user who posted the tweet, participants were less likely to do so if the content conflicted with their beliefs. This suggested that while people are drawn to confront opposing views, they avoid prolonged exposure to such content.

Mochon and Schwartz also tested whether the way a message is framed affects the likelihood of engaging with ideology-inconsistent content. Their fourth study was conducted using Facebook again, targeting users with liberal or conservative posts about gun control. The posts were framed either as information-based (low threat) or action-based (high threat). For example, a low-threat post might provide facts about gun control, while a high-threat post might call for immediate action to change gun laws. The researchers measured the engagement rates, including clicks and comments, for each type of post.

The findings revealed that posts framed as high-threat (fighting for a cause) generated more engagement from users with opposing views than low-threat (informational) posts. This suggested that the emotional intensity of the message plays a key role in driving the confrontation effect, with more provocative content leading to greater engagement.

In Study 5, the researchers investigated whether the importance of the topic being discussed affects the confrontation effect. The hypothesis was that people would be more likely to engage with ideology-inconsistent content on important topics, like COVID-19 vaccines, than on less personally relevant topics, such as smartphone brands.

Participants (803 individuals from Prolific) were randomly assigned to read tweets about either COVID-19 vaccines (a high-importance topic) or smartphone brands (a low-importance topic). As in previous studies, participants were asked to comment on ideology-consistent or inconsistent tweets, and their emotional reactions were measured.

The results showed that participants were more likely to engage with opposing views on important topics like vaccines, but not on less important topics like smartphones. This finding indicated that the confrontation effect is stronger when the issue at hand is personally or socially significant, providing a boundary condition for the effect.

Finally, in their fifth study, Mochon and Schwartz examined whether confronting ideology-inconsistent content (by commenting on it) could reduce the emotional distress or outrage people experience. Prior research suggests that people might feel better after expressing their negative emotions, so the researchers tested whether commenting on an opposing viewpoint would lower the outrage participants felt.

In this study, 400 participants from Prolific were exposed to political tweets from either Joe Biden or Donald Trump. Some participants were given the opportunity to comment on the tweet, while others were not. The researchers measured their levels of outrage and satisfaction both before and after exposure to the tweet.

The findings showed that participants who were able to comment on the ideology-inconsistent tweet experienced a reduction in their outrage levels, compared to those who were not given the chance to respond. This suggests that part of the reason people engage with opposing views online is to relieve their emotional discomfort.

“Outrage is a powerful driver of online engagement, often leading users to interact with content they would prefer to avoid,” Mochon told PsyPost. “Users should be more mindful of their reactions to online content and whether it is in their best interest to engage with it.”

Despite its important insights, the study also had limitations. One of the main challenges was the complexity of measuring engagement across different types of online platforms. Each social media platform has unique features that may influence how users engage with content, and the study focused on specific actions like clicking or commenting.

Future research could explore other forms of engagement, such as sharing or following, to gain a fuller understanding of how people interact with ideology-inconsistent content. Additionally, the study largely focused on political content, leaving room for further exploration of how the confrontation effect operates in non-political contexts.

“Social media is a complex and constantly changing environment,” Mochon said. “While outrage is a strong driver of engagement, the specific behaviors it leads to online may depend on the platform and social content.”

Future research could also explore whether this effect varies across cultures or social groups, as well as how it might evolve with changing social media dynamics. There may be other emotions besides outrage, such as fear or sadness, that drive engagement with opposing viewpoints. Understanding these nuances could help researchers develop more comprehensive theories about how people interact with conflicting information online.

“We would like to understand how to help people engage more constructively and reduce the amount of toxic content online,” Mochon said.

The study, “The confrontation effect: When users engage more with ideology-inconsistent content online,” was published October 5, 2024.

RELATED

Study finds Trump and Harris used distinct rhetoric in 2024—but shared more similarities than expected
Political Psychology

Study finds Trump and Harris used distinct rhetoric in 2024—but shared more similarities than expected

August 24, 2025

Donald Trump and Kamala Harris framed the 2024 presidential debate in starkly different terms, according to a new study—but their language also showed surprising overlap in tone, emotional content, and specificity.

Read moreDetails
The most popular dementia videos on TikTok tend to have the lowest quality, study find
Social Media

Most TikTok videos about birth control are unreliable, study finds

August 23, 2025

TikTok is flooded with misleading content about contraception, according to a new study. Most viral videos are not made by medical experts and often promote “natural” methods while casting doubt on hormonal options and professional medical advice.

Read moreDetails
Americans broadly agree on what’s “woke,” but partisan cues still shape perceptions
Political Psychology

Americans broadly agree on what’s “woke,” but partisan cues still shape perceptions

August 22, 2025

Do Americans agree on what “woke” means? A new study suggests yes—up to a point. The term tends to signal different things depending on political identity, especially around race, gender, and alignment with the Democratic Party.

Read moreDetails
Narcissistic grandiosity predicts greater involvement in LGBTQ activism
Moral Psychology

New psychology research finds leftist causes widely seen as more moral — even by conservatives

August 21, 2025

A pair of studies conducted in Spain provides evidence of a striking moral asymmetry in politics: both leftists and rightists feel more morally obligated to defend progressive causes, and conservatives tend to view liberals as more morally upright than vice versa.

Read moreDetails
People high in psychopathy and low in cognitive ability are the most politically active online, study finds
Political Psychology

People high in psychopathy and low in cognitive ability are the most politically active online, study finds

August 20, 2025

New research highlights a striking pattern: individuals with high psychopathic traits and lower cognitive ability tend to be the most politically active online. The study also links fear of missing out to digital engagement across eight diverse national contexts.

Read moreDetails
The brain is shown with a wave of sound
Neuroimaging

Early brain responses to political leaders’ faces appear unaffected by partisanship

August 15, 2025

New research suggests that while the brain quickly distinguishes politicians from strangers, it doesn’t initially register political allegiance. The findings challenge assumptions about how early partisan bias kicks in during perception and suggest that party loyalty may emerge later.

Read moreDetails
People with narcissistic tendencies report more ostracism and are more often excluded
Political Psychology

Intellectual humility is linked to less political and religious polarization across the board

August 10, 2025

A large online study indicates that intellectual humility is linked to less hostility toward political and religious opponents. The effect was seen across political parties and belief systems, and persisted even after controlling for the strength of participants’ convictions.

Read moreDetails
Antagonistic narcissism and psychopathic tendencies predict left-wing antihierarchical aggression, study finds
Political Psychology

Populism may act as a “thermometer” for democratic health

August 8, 2025

Long-term data from Britain and the Netherlands reveal that citizens’ populist beliefs rise and fall alongside changes in democratic satisfaction. The research challenges the idea that populist attitudes are static traits and highlights their potential responsiveness to political reforms.

Read moreDetails

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Insecurely attached individuals are less likely to go for a compromise in relationship conflicts

Conspiracy theories can significantly influence public support for war

New psychology research identifies factors that predict sexual dream intensity

Can facial fillers and Botox-like injections improve your dating prospects or how others see you?

A simple cognitive vaccine can make you more resistant to misinformation

Psychopathic men and less selective women report more sex via Tinder

Letting loose with a swear word may actually make you stronger

Pilates may help treat female sexual dysfunction, new study indicates

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy