Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Definitions

Understanding Classical vs. Operant Conditioning

by Eric W. Dolan
March 19, 2025
in Definitions
[PsyPost]

[PsyPost]

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Follow PsyPost on Google News

Classical conditioning and operant conditioning are two fundamental concepts in psychology that explain how we learn from our environment. Both involve learning associations, but they work in different ways. Classical conditioning is about associating two stimuli (for example, a sound and food) to elicit an automatic response, whereas operant conditioning is about associating a behavior and its consequence (a reward or punishment) to influence whether that behavior happens again. This article explores the differences between these forms of learning, tracing their historical roots with Ivan Pavlov and B.F. Skinner, and provides real-world examples of how they shape human and animal behavior today.

Classical Conditioning: Pavlov’s Accidental Discovery

Classical conditioning was first discovered by Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov in the early 1900s. Pavlov wasn’t initially studying learning at all – he was researching digestion in dogs (a line of work that even earned him the 1904 Nobel Prize) when he noticed something curious. His lab dogs would start salivating not only when they tasted food, but even before – for instance, when they saw the lab assistant who usually fed them. This was puzzling because salivation is a reflexive, automatic response to food, not to a person walking into the room.

Intrigued, Pavlov designed a series of experiments to investigate this phenomenon. He began pairing a neutral stimulus (such as a sound) with the presentation of food. In one famous setup, he rang a bell (or in some trials, a metronome) and then immediately gave the dog food. Initially, the bell meant nothing to the dog, but after several repetitions, the dog started to drool simply upon hearing the bell, even if no food followed.

In other words, the dog had learned that the bell predicted food and responded as if food was on the way. Pavlov found that by associating the sound of a bell with the taste of food, dogs would begin to salivate at the sound alone. He called this learned anticipation a “conditioned reflex.” The previously neutral bell became a conditioned stimulus that evoked a conditioned response (salivation), illustrating what we now call classical conditioning.

A dog salivating in anticipation of a treat – an example of classical conditioning. Pavlov’s experiments showed that dogs could be conditioned to drool at the sound of a bell after it was repeatedly paired with food. In classical conditioning, an automatic reflex (like salivation) becomes triggered by a new cue after the cue is associated with an original trigger (food). (Credit: Yohan euan o4 via Wikimedia Commons)

Pavlov’s findings were groundbreaking. They revealed that organisms can learn by association: if one stimulus reliably precedes another, a subject can begin to respond to the first as if it were the second. This form of learning turned out to apply not just to dogs, but to many organisms – including humans. For example, psychologist John B. Watson famously showed that emotional reactions could be conditioned in people.

In a 1920 experiment, little Albert (an infant in Watson’s study) initially had no fear of a white rat. Watson and his colleague Rosalie Rayner then repeatedly made a loud, frightening noise each time the baby saw the rat. After a few such pairings, the child began to cry just upon seeing the white rat, even without the noise. He had learned to associate the rat with the scary noise, demonstrating that even fears and other emotions can be acquired through classical conditioning. This explains how a harmless thing can come to trigger fear or other feelings – for instance, someone might develop a phobia of dogs after being bitten once, because they start associating the sight of dogs with the pain and fear of the bite.

Everyday Examples of Classical Conditioning

Classical conditioning often happens in our daily lives without us realizing it. Have you ever felt nervous just sitting in a dentist’s waiting room because you associate it with the pain of past procedures? That’s classical conditioning at work – the waiting room is a neutral place, but through association with unpleasant dental work it becomes a trigger for anxiety.

Similarly, certain smells or songs can evoke vivid memories and feelings. For example, the smell of chlorine might instantly take you back to happy summers at the pool, or a particular song might make you feel nostalgic because it was playing during a meaningful event in your life. Advertisers also take advantage of classical conditioning: a commercial might pair a product with upbeat music or attractive imagery so that we come to feel positive about the product (the previously neutral stimulus) by association. In all these cases, we respond involuntarily with feelings or reflexes that we’ve learned to associate with something else.

Operant Conditioning: Skinner’s Behavior Shaping

If classical conditioning is about involuntary responses (like reflexes or emotions) triggered by new cues, operant conditioning is about voluntary behaviors and their consequences. This concept was developed by American psychologist B.F. Skinner, who expanded on earlier ideas from Edward Thorndike. Thorndike had formulated the law of effect – the observation that behaviors followed by satisfying outcomes are more likely to be repeated, while those followed by unpleasant outcomes are less likely to be repeated.

Skinner took this idea and ran with it. He believed that most of our behaviors are influenced by the consequences that follow them. In the 1930s, Skinner began scientifically testing how consequences shape behavior by conducting experiments with animals, mainly rats and pigeons. He built an apparatus called the Skinner box (operant conditioning chamber) – essentially a small box equipped with a lever or button, a food dispenser, and sometimes lights or sounds. When the animal inside presses the lever (a voluntary action), it can receive a food pellet reward. Skinner would observe how changing the consequences (giving a reward, not giving it, or even administering a mild punishment like a slight shock) affected the animal’s behavior.

The core idea of operant conditioning is simple but powerful: behaviors that are reinforced tend to increase, while behaviors that are punished tend to decrease. Reinforcement means the consequence makes the behavior more likely in the future – this could be giving something positive (called positive reinforcement, like a treat or praise) or removing something negative (called negative reinforcement, like stopping a loud noise when the animal does the right thing).

Punishment is the opposite – it makes the behavior less likely to occur again, either by adding an unpleasant outcome (positive punishment, like a scolding or shock) or taking away something desirable (negative punishment, like confiscating a toy). Unlike classical conditioning, which links two stimuli, operant conditioning links a behavior with its result. The learner is active, choosing to perform (or not perform) a behavior based on its past consequences.

A rat in a Skinner box (operant conditioning chamber). In such experiments, the rat may press a lever and receive a food pellet as a reward. B.F. Skinner used these boxes to demonstrate how consequences (like rewards or punishments) shape voluntary behavior. When a behavior is followed by a rewarding outcome, the animal is more likely to repeat that behavior – a clear example of operant conditioning in action. (Credit: AndreasJS, CC BY-SA 4.0, via Wikimedia Commons)

Skinner’s research showed how remarkably adaptive behavior can be. For instance, a rat that accidentally presses a lever and gets food will press it again, and soon it learns to press the lever whenever it’s hungry. Skinner also worked with pigeons – he could shape their actions by rewarding successive approximations of a target behavior (a process called shaping).

By doing so, he managed to teach pigeons to do surprisingly complex things. In one famous demonstration, Skinner used shaping to train pigeons to play ping-pong – the birds learned to hit a tiny ball back and forth for a food reward! This might sound amusing, but it underscored a serious point: through operant conditioning, even new and non-instinctive behaviors can be learned if reinforced step by step. Animal trainers today still use these principles – whether it’s teaching dogs to fetch or dolphins to do tricks, rewarding incremental successes is key.

Everyday Examples of Operant Conditioning

Operant conditioning is constantly at play in everyday life, because much of our behavior is guided by past rewards and punishments. Consider how parents teach young children right from wrong. If a child cleans their room and gets praised or a treat, that positive reinforcement makes it more likely the child will clean up again in the future.

Conversely, if the child hits a sibling and is put in time-out (a form of punishment by removing them from play), that behavior is likely to decrease. In workplaces, operant conditioning is evident when employees work hard to receive bonuses, promotions, or even just a paycheck – as Skinner would say, the salary is reinforcing the behavior of coming to work.

On the flip side, getting a speeding ticket (a punishment) tends to make us drive more cautiously to avoid future fines. Even our personal habits can be shaped this way. For example, if you set a goal to exercise regularly and you reward yourself with a favorite smoothie after each workout, you’re using positive reinforcement to make yourself more motivated to exercise. Over time, the behavior becomes a habit because it’s been consistently rewarded.

Key Differences Between Classical and Operant Conditioning

While both classical and operant conditioning involve learning from experience, they differ in what is being learned and how the learning occurs. Here are some key differences:

  • Involuntary vs. Voluntary: Classical conditioning usually involves involuntary, automatic responses (reflexes or emotions). The subject doesn’t need to actively do anything; the response (like salivation or fear) just happens after the stimulus. Operant conditioning involves voluntary behaviors – the subject chooses to act in a certain way (pressing a lever, cleaning a room) to obtain a reward or avoid punishment. In classical conditioning the organism is more passive (learning by association), whereas in operant conditioning the organism is active (learning by doing).
  • What’s Being Associated: In classical conditioning, an organism learns an association between two stimuli – one stimulus (like a bell) comes to predict another stimulus (like food). The learned behavior is a reflex triggered by the first stimulus. In operant conditioning, the association is between a behavior and its consequence – the organism learns that a certain action will result in a certain outcome (reward or punishment). Rather than triggering an existing reflex, operant conditioning leads to new behaviors or changes in the frequency of behaviors based on outcomes.
  • Timing: In classical conditioning, the neutral stimulus typically occurs just before the unconditioned stimulus (the bell rings before the food arrives) so that the organism comes to expect the second stimulus after the first. In operant conditioning, the reinforcement or punishment comes after the behavior, as a consequence of it. Essentially, classical conditioning is about what happens before a reflex, whereas operant conditioning is about what happens after a voluntary action.
  • Extinction: Both types of learning can fade if the associations stop. In classical conditioning, if the conditioned stimulus (e.g. bell) is presented many times without the unconditioned stimulus (food), eventually the learned response (salivation) will diminish – a process called extinction. In operant conditioning, if a behavior that was reinforced stops being reinforced, the behavior will gradually stop as well. For example, if a slot machine stops paying out (no more reward), a person will eventually quit playing.
Comparison of classical and operant conditioning. Classical conditioning links stimuli to responses, while operant conditioning reinforces or punishes behaviors to shape future actions. (Credit: Perey, CC BY-SA 4.0, via Wikimedia Commons)

Despite these differences, it’s important to note that classical and operant conditioning aren’t mutually exclusive – both processes can be occurring in the same situation. For instance, consider a pet dog: it may learn to come running at the sound of a crinkling treat bag (classical conditioning, associating a sound with food) and also learn to sit on command because it gets a treat when it does so (operant conditioning, performing a behavior for a reward).

Why These Forms of Learning Matter Today

Understanding classical and operant conditioning isn’t just a theoretical exercise from old psychology textbooks – these principles have many practical applications in modern life. Therapists use classical conditioning principles to help people overcome phobias by gradually associating the feared object with calm, safe experiences (a technique known as desensitization). Classical conditioning also plays a role in treating disorders like PTSD and addiction, by breaking associations that trigger harmful reactions.

Operant conditioning principles are widely used in education and parenting, under the banner of behavior modification – for example, teachers might use token reward systems to encourage participation and good behavior in class, and parents use praise (or time-outs) to guide their children’s actions. Animal training, as mentioned, relies heavily on operant conditioning: from guide dogs learning to aid people with disabilities to dolphins learning rescue tasks, reward-based training is the norm. Even our personal self-improvement efforts (like sticking to an exercise routine or productive schedule) often succeed or fail based on how we reward or discipline ourselves.

In summary, classical conditioning (pioneered by Pavlov) teaches us how creatures can form associations between events and thus anticipate what’s coming, while operant conditioning (championed by Skinner) teaches us how outcomes shape future actions. These two forms of conditioning provide a foundation for understanding a huge range of behaviors.

From a dog salivating at the sound of a bell to a student studying hard to earn good grades, the principles discovered by Pavlov and Skinner continue to influence both psychology as a science and the everyday techniques we use to train, teach, and even manipulate behavior. By recognizing these conditioning processes in daily life, we become more aware of how experiences shape our reactions and habits – and that awareness is the first step in learning how to change them if we need to.

RELATED

What is psychology?
Definitions

What is psychology?

July 3, 2025

Why do people do what they do? Psychology seeks the answers.

Read moreDetails
Soul ties: Exploring a popular belief in deep emotional bonds
Definitions

Soul ties: Exploring a popular belief in deep emotional bonds

March 22, 2025

What exactly are soul ties? A popular concept suggests some emotional bonds go deeper than others—possibly to the soul.

Read moreDetails
Negative reinforcement in psychology: Everything you need to know
Definitions

Negative reinforcement in psychology: Everything you need to know

March 21, 2025

Negative reinforcement is one of the core concepts in behavioral psychology, yet it’s often misunderstood.

Read moreDetails
The grey rock method: A strategy to disarm narcissists and toxic people
Definitions

The grey rock method: A strategy to disarm narcissists and toxic people

March 19, 2025

Learn how the Grey Rock Method can neutralize toxic interactions.

Read moreDetails
Definitions

Psychology vs. Sociology: Uncovering the Nature of Mind and Society

January 27, 2024

In the vast expanse of social sciences, two disciplines stand prominently: psychology and sociology. Often perceived as overlapping areas of study, they both seek to unravel the complexities of human behavior and social interactions. However, beneath this shared goal lies a world of difference in perspectives, methodologies, and areas of...

Read moreDetails
Entheogens vs. Psychedelics: What is The Difference?
Definitions

Entheogens vs. Psychedelics: What is The Difference?

January 26, 2024

"Entheogens" and "psychedelics" refer to overlapping groups of substances, yet they are defined differently based on context and usage.

Read moreDetails
Definitions

Definition: What is intrasexual competition?

January 26, 2024

Intrasexual competition refers to the rivalry between members of the same sex, often for access to mates. This phenomenon, observed across various species, including humans, is a cornerstone of Darwin's theory of sexual selection. In simple terms, it's the competition among individuals of one gender, typically over resources that would...

Read moreDetails
Implicit vs. Explicit Memory | Definition, Examples, & History
Definitions

Implicit vs. Explicit Memory | Definition, Examples, & History

January 26, 2024

Memory, a cornerstone of human cognition, has long intrigued both scientists and laypeople. Its intricate workings underpin everything from our basic survival instincts to the complexities of human culture. Central to understanding memory are two distinct types: implicit and explicit memories. Let's unravel these concepts, providing clarity through definitions and...

Read moreDetails

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Microplastics found to obstruct brain blood vessels in troubling neuroscience study

Scientists map the visual patterns people use when evaluating others’ bodies

The ADHD symptom no one talks about: rejection sensitive dysphoria

How stress affects us may depend on the balance between testosterone and cortisol

Women who fake orgasm tend to struggle with emotional clarity and impulse control, study finds

What we know about a person changes how our brain processes their face

Dementia diagnoses are often delayed for years, new study finds

Are “zombie cells” in your blood vessels driving long-COVID and chronic fatigue?

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy