Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology Social Media

Spontaneous mind wandering linked to heavier social smartphone use

by Eric W. Dolan
July 23, 2025
in Social Media
[Adobe Stock]

[Adobe Stock]

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Don't miss out! Follow PsyPost on Bluesky!

New research published in Psychological Reports indicates that people who frequently experience spontaneous mind wandering also tend to be more preoccupied with the online world and use their smartphones more for social purposes. The findings suggest that this link is influenced by a mental tendency called online vigilance, and that mindfulness might weaken the connection.

The authors of the new study wanted to better understand how smartphone use relates to attention. They were especially interested in how different types of mind wandering—spontaneous and deliberate—relate to smartphone behavior.

Past studies found that people who mind-wander more also report higher smartphone use, but these were based on surveys rather than real-world use data. The current research aimed to investigate whether these findings would hold true using objective, recorded smartphone activity, and to explore how online vigilance and mindfulness might explain or influence the connection.

The study focused on U.S. undergraduate students and took place during the COVID-19 pandemic, a time when many students were learning remotely and likely using smartphones heavily. Researchers recruited 188 iPhone users to take part in an online survey. Participants completed several questionnaires assessing their tendency to mind-wander, their overall mindfulness, and their level of online vigilance. They also provided screenshots of their iPhone screen time settings, which showed their actual weekly smartphone use across different app categories.

To measure mind wandering, the researchers used both a general questionnaire and scales that separated spontaneous (unintentional) and deliberate (intentional) mind wandering. They also measured mindfulness using a well-established inventory suited for people without meditation experience. Online vigilance was assessed with a 12-item scale that captured how much participants think about, monitor, and react to online content.

The smartphone data captured average daily use, number of pickups and notifications, and weekly use across app categories such as social media, entertainment, games, education, and health. Researchers paid special attention to the “Social” category, which includes apps used for messaging and viewing others’ content, like Instagram and TikTok.

The results showed that participants spent more time in the Social app category than any other. On average, they spent over 17 hours per week using social media, significantly more than time spent on entertainment or productivity apps. This pattern mirrors findings from other studies and reflects the central role social media plays in the daily lives of many college students.

The most notable findings involved the relationships among spontaneous mind wandering, online vigilance, and social smartphone use. Participants who scored higher in spontaneous mind wandering also had higher levels of online vigilance. In turn, those with higher online vigilance reported greater use of social apps.

This suggests that when people frequently have self-generated thoughts unrelated to their current task—particularly those that arise unintentionally—they may become more mentally preoccupied with their online lives, leading them to use their smartphones more for social purposes.

Statistical analysis confirmed that online vigilance helped explain the link between spontaneous mind wandering and social smartphone use. That is, people who tend to mind-wander spontaneously may be more mentally tuned in to the online world, which then predicts more time spent on social apps. This pattern was not observed for deliberate mind wandering, which involves intentionally letting the mind drift. This distinction highlights that unintentional mental drift may be more strongly tied to smartphone habits than purposeful daydreaming.

The researchers also explored the role of mindfulness. Although mindfulness did not directly relate to how much time people spent on their smartphones, it did influence the strength of the relationship between online vigilance and social smartphone use. People with higher levels of mindfulness were less likely to translate their online vigilance into increased social smartphone use. In other words, even if someone had frequent online thoughts, those with higher mindfulness appeared more able to resist the impulse to engage with their smartphones.

This protective effect of mindfulness was especially evident among participants with the highest mindfulness scores. For these individuals, high levels of online vigilance did not lead to significantly higher social smartphone use, suggesting that mindfulness may offer some buffer against technology overuse driven by mental preoccupation.

In addition to testing their main model, the researchers explored whether smartphone notifications—the external pings and alerts that draw users back in—might also relate to increased smartphone use. They found that notifications were linked to both the number of phone pickups and social app use. There was also some evidence that notifications might influence social smartphone use indirectly by sparking spontaneous mind wandering, which then increased online vigilance. However, this pathway appeared weaker than the main findings linking internal mental states to smartphone behavior.

However, because the study used a correlational design, the researchers cannot determine whether spontaneous mind wandering causes online vigilance or social smartphone use, or whether the direction goes the other way. It’s possible that frequent smartphone use could increase online vigilance and lead to more spontaneous mind wandering. In fact, additional analyses supported this reverse pathway as a plausible explanation. The researchers also did not measure the content of participants’ mind-wandering thoughts, so it remains unclear how many of them were actually about social media or online activities.

Future research could use experience sampling methods—brief surveys sent to participants’ phones throughout the day—to track real-time thoughts and behaviors more accurately. Experimental studies could also investigate how people with different levels of mind wandering or mindfulness respond in controlled settings that simulate real-life distractions.

The study, “Spontaneous Mind Wandering and Smartphone Use,” was authored by Lara L. Jones, Gregory A. Norville, and Zachary I. Wunder.

RELATED

Daughters who feel more attractive report stronger, more protective bonds with their fathers
Social Media

Moral outrage spreads petitions online—but doesn’t always inspire people to sign them

July 15, 2025

Posts expressing moral outrage were more likely to go viral but did not lead to more petition signatures, while posts using agentic, group identity, or prosocial language attracted more signatures despite receiving less online engagement.

Read moreDetails
The most popular dementia videos on TikTok tend to have the lowest quality, study find
Addiction

People with short-video addiction show altered brain responses during decision-making

July 8, 2025

People who frequently use short-video apps like TikTok may show reduced loss sensitivity and impulsive decision-making, according to a new neuroimaging study that links addictive use patterns to changes in brain activity during risky choices.

Read moreDetails
Scientists show how you’re unknowingly sealing yourself in an information bubble
Cognitive Science

Scientists show how you’re unknowingly sealing yourself in an information bubble

June 29, 2025

Scientists have found that belief polarization doesn’t always come from misinformation or social media bubbles. Instead, it often begins with a simple search. Our choice of words—and the algorithm’s response—can subtly seal us inside our own informational comfort zones.

Read moreDetails
TikTok tics study sheds light on recovery trends and ongoing mental health challenges
Body Image and Body Dysmorphia

TikTok and similar platforms linked to body dissatisfaction and eating disorder symptoms

June 27, 2025

Frequent use of platforms like TikTok and YouTube Shorts is linked to disordered eating symptoms among teens, according to new research. The study found that body comparisons and dissatisfaction may help explain this troubling association—especially among girls.

Read moreDetails
Loneliness skews partner perceptions, harming relationships and reinforcing isolation
Mental Health

Maximization style and social media addiction linked to relationship obsessive compulsive disorder

June 24, 2025

Researchers have identified connections between obsessive thoughts about relationships, emotional closeness, and habits like social media addiction and striving for perfection. The findings highlight risk factors that can deepen doubt and tension in romantic connections, especially when conflict is present.

Read moreDetails
It’s not digital illiteracy: Here’s why older adults are drawn to dubious news
Social Media

Believing “news will find me” is linked to sharing fake news, study finds

June 22, 2025

People who rely on social media to “stumble upon” news are more prone to spreading misinformation, according to a new longitudinal study.

Read moreDetails
Dark personality traits and specific humor styles are linked to online trolling, study finds
Artificial Intelligence

Memes can serve as strong indicators of coming mass violence

June 15, 2025

A new study finds that surges in visual propaganda—like memes and doctored images—often precede political violence. By combining AI with expert analysis, researchers tracked manipulated content leading up to Russia’s invasion of Ukraine, revealing early warning signs of instability.

Read moreDetails
Democrats dislike Republicans more than Republicans dislike Democrats, studies find
Political Psychology

TikTok “edits” can reshape how voters see politicians, study suggests

June 11, 2025

A new study finds that TikTok-style “edits” portraying politicians as attractive or powerful can sway public perceptions, boosting attractiveness and favorability—especially for Donald Trump. These videos may signal a shift in how political influence is crafted and consumed.

Read moreDetails

Leave a Reply Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Only premium subscribers can comment — log in or join now.

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Individuals with alcohol use disorder have much higher concentration of glutathione in certain brain areas

Humans still beat AI at one key creative task, new study finds

Study shows Congressional stock gains come at democracy’s expense

Psychedelics alter neurochemical signals tied to hunger and mood in the hypothalamus

Zapping the brain’s prefrontal cortex with electricity helps people learn math

Brain scans shed light on how green space might support children’s cognitive development

New study shows brain circuit disruption mimics antidepressant effects

Individuals adhering to ketogenic diet are less likely to suffer from depression

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy