Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

Moderate aerobic exercise enhances the brain’s “eighth sense”

by Eric W. Dolan
August 8, 2025
in Cognitive Science
[Adobe Stock]

[Adobe Stock]

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Follow PsyPost on Google News

A new study published in Psychology of Sport and Exercise shows that regular aerobic exercise not only improves mood but also strengthens people’s ability to sense what’s happening inside their bodies—a skill known as interoception. In a 12-week intervention, young adults who previously led sedentary lifestyles showed increased accuracy in perceiving their own heartbeats, greater confidence in those perceptions, and reduced symptoms of depression and anxiety.

Exercise is widely recognized for its ability to enhance emotional well-being, and it’s often recommended as a strategy to reduce symptoms of anxiety and depression. But while many studies focus on external improvements like energy levels or fitness, fewer have explored how exercise might influence internal awareness.

Interoception—the sense of internal bodily signals like heartbeat or breathing—is sometimes referred to as the “eighth sense.” In addition to the traditional five senses, scientists often include proprioception (body position) and vestibular sensation (balance) as sixth and seventh.

Interoception rounds out this expanded list as the sense responsible for detecting the physiological condition of the body—things like hunger, thirst, heart rate, breathing, or the need to use the bathroom. It helps people stay in tune with what’s happening inside them, which can shape emotional experiences, guide decisions, and support self-regulation. Impaired interoception has been linked to several psychological conditions, including anxiety, depression, and eating disorders.

The researchers wanted to investigate whether regular aerobic exercise could strengthen this internal sense—and whether those changes might be accompanied by improvements in mental health.

“Regular physical exercise is known to benefit mental health, but the underlying physiological mechanisms remain unclear. We were particularly interested in whether exercise improves cardioception—our ability to perceive our own heartbeats,” said study author Tomasz S. Ligeza, a postdoctoral researcher affiliated with the Institute of Psychology at Jagiellonian University in Kraków.

“Since aerobic (cardio) training directly engages the cardiovascular system, we hypothesized that it could also enhance ‘cardio awareness.’ Moreover, most prior research on the exercise–mental health relationship has focused on clinical or older populations, so we wanted to examine whether these effects also apply to healthy but physically inactive young adults—a population that has grown recently.”

The study involved 62 young adults between the ages of 18 and 32 who were not physically active at the start of the research. Participants were randomly assigned to either an exercise group or a control group that continued their usual habits. The exercise group completed a supervised 12-week cycling program on stationary bikes. The first six weeks involved moderate, continuous cycling; the following six weeks used a more demanding interval approach with alternating high and low intensities.

Participants’ interoceptive accuracy (IAcc) was measured using the heartbeat counting task, in which they tried to count their own heartbeats without physically checking their pulse. After each trial, they rated their confidence in their performance, giving researchers a measure of interoceptive confidence (ICon). Depression and anxiety symptoms were assessed using standardized self-report questionnaires, along with a scale measuring general self-efficacy—a person’s belief in their ability to succeed at tasks and handle challenges.

These assessments took place at three points: before the program began, after six weeks, and after the full 12 weeks. Throughout the intervention, the exercise group completed three training sessions per week, and both groups wore activity trackers to monitor adherence.

The exercise group showed clear improvements in interoception and emotional well-being after just six weeks. Participants became better at detecting their heartbeats (higher IAcc) and reported feeling more confident in their ability to do so (higher ICon). These improvements held steady through the remainder of the study but did not increase further after the shift to more intense training. By contrast, the control group showed no change in interoceptive accuracy at any point in the study.

Mood-related outcomes followed a similar pattern. The exercise group reported reduced symptoms of depression and anxiety after six weeks, and those improvements were maintained—but not amplified—after completing the second half of the program. Their scores on the self-efficacy scale also increased, indicating a stronger belief in their own capabilities. The control group, on the other hand, showed no meaningful changes in mood or self-efficacy during the 12-week period.

“Our 12-week program had two phases: the first with moderate-intensity exercise, the second with higher intensity,” Ligeza said. “We expected greater improvements after the second phase, but that wasn’t the case. It’s possible we reached a ceiling effect—meaning that in healthy young adults, further increases in intensity (or a longer program) might not bring additional benefits for mood or cardioception.”

These findings suggest that moderate aerobic exercise is enough to produce meaningful psychological and physiological changes, and that higher-intensity workouts do not necessarily add further benefits—at least in terms of internal bodily awareness or mood in young adults.

“Exercise improves both mental health and aspects of body awareness—and the good news is, you don’t need much,” Ligeza told PsyPost. “Just three sessions per week of moderate-intensity activity like cycling, for 30 to 60 minutes, can be enough. So if someone is physically inactive and unsure whether it’s worth starting, this study should serve as a friendly scientific nudge to get moving.”

But why does interoception matter? Previous research indicates that interoception plays an important role in emotional life. The ability to perceive internal states like heart rate or breathing contributes to how people experience and regulate emotions. Better interoceptive accuracy has been linked to improved emotional insight, decision-making, and mental health. Conversely, disrupted interoception has been associated with conditions like depression, anxiety, eating disorders, and chronic pain.

The study has some limitations. The control group was entirely passive, meaning participants did not engage in any structured activity during the study. While this design allowed researchers to compare natural lifestyle conditions, it also meant that differences between groups may reflect not just exercise but other factors, such as time spent in a structured routine.

Another limitation is that the study was not designed to determine whether improved interoception caused the improvements in mood—or whether both were separate effects of the exercise program. Although the similar patterns across measures suggest a possible link, the researchers were not able to test this directly. Future studies could help clarify whether better interoception leads to better emotional regulation, or whether the relationship works in the other direction—or both.

Looking forward, “we aim to better understand the psychophysiological mechanisms through which physical activity influences mental functioning,” Ligeza explained. “Another goal is to explore more personalized exercise prescriptions, so that individuals can maximize the mental and physiological benefits of training.”

The study, “Long-term aerobic exercise enhances interoception and reduces symptoms of depression and anxiety in physically inactive young adults: A randomized controlled trial,” was authored by Michał Remiszewski, Gabriela Rajtar, Zuzanna Komarek, Tomasz Pałka, Marcin Maciejczyk, and Tomasz S. Ligeza.

RELATED

Neuroscientists identify a reversible biological mechanism behind drug-induced cognitive deficits
Cognitive Science

Dopamine’s role in learning may be broader than previously thought

August 11, 2025

New research reveals dopamine helps the brain juggle fast, flexible problem-solving with gradual habit formation. By boosting working memory use and enhancing trial-and-error learning, dopamine influences both how quickly we learn and how costly mental effort feels.

Read moreDetails
Exercise can reduce feelings of hopelessness among patients in suicide crisis, pilot study finds
Cognitive Science

Physically active individuals tend to have slightly better cognitive abilities on average

August 10, 2025

A new meta-analysis suggests that while physical activity generally has a small positive impact on cognition, outdoor moderate-to-vigorous exercise paired with cognitively challenging activities delivers the biggest gains across age groups, from preschoolers to older adults.

Read moreDetails
People with narcissistic tendencies report more ostracism and are more often excluded
Anxiety

Sleep may amplify negative memory bias in anxious youth

August 10, 2025

Researchers have found that children and young adolescents with higher anxiety tend to generalize negative memories more after sleep, raising questions about how nighttime memory processes could reinforce anxiety-related thought patterns during a sensitive developmental stage.

Read moreDetails
Moderate aerobic exercise enhances the brain’s “eighth sense”
Cognitive Science

Bright children from low-income homes lose cognitive edge in early secondary school

August 8, 2025

A new UK study finds that bright 5-year-olds from low-income families match their affluent peers academically through primary school, but between ages 11 and 14, they face steep declines in motivation, behavior, mental health, and exam performance.

Read moreDetails
What we know about a person changes how our brain processes their face
Cognitive Science

New research links higher copper intake to better cognitive functioning in older adults

August 6, 2025

A new study suggests that older adults who consume more copper-rich foods—such as shellfish, nuts, and dark chocolate—tend to perform better on memory and attention tests, highlighting a possible link between dietary copper and cognitive health.

Read moreDetails
Scientists show how you’re unknowingly sealing yourself in an information bubble
Cognitive Science

Cognitive fixation from Google searches hurts a team’s ability to innovate

August 3, 2025

If your team's brainstorming sessions feel stale, your internet connection might be the culprit. Research reveals a hidden cost to online idea generation: it steers everyone toward the same common answers, stifling the collective innovation.

Read moreDetails
People with higher intelligence make more accurate predictions about their lifespan
Cognitive Science

People with higher intelligence make more accurate predictions about their lifespan

August 2, 2025

New research helps explain why higher intelligence is linked to better life outcomes. The study shows that people with higher IQs form more accurate and stable beliefs about the world, leading to more realistic predictions and better decision-making.

Read moreDetails
What we know about a person changes how our brain processes their face
Cognitive Science

What we know about a person changes how our brain processes their face

August 1, 2025

Researchers have found that social knowledge shapes how the brain represents familiar faces. Participants who understood a character’s story showed stronger brain activity in regions linked to identity and memory, even though everyone saw the same faces the same amount.

Read moreDetails

Leave a Reply Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Only premium subscribers can comment — log in or join now.

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

The science-backed case for doing nothing: why your brain needs time to drift

Dopamine’s role in learning may be broader than previously thought

What really keeps people committed to long-term goals? This study offers a key insight

Psychedelic experiences may offer a lasting boost in perceived life meaning

Inflammation levels may shape how cannabis affects anxiety and sleep

Assimilation-induced dehumanization: Psychology research uncovers a dark side effect of AI

Dementia rates vary sharply across U.S. regions

Intellectual humility is linked to less political and religious polarization across the board

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy