Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

People who weigh more than others see distances as farther away

by Eric W. Dolan
March 12, 2016
in Cognitive Science
Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

Research conducted by psychologists at Purdue University and Colorado State has found that a person’s perception of distances is influenced by their physical body weight.

The study, titled “Perceived distance and obesity: It’s what you weigh, not what you think” was published in the March issue of the scientific journal Acta Psychologica. The research was conducted by Mila Sugovica, Philip Turkb, Jessica K. Witt.

“One common assumption is that people who struggle with obesity make poor behavioral and lifestyle choices,” the researchers wrote, noting that obese individuals are more likely to drive — rather than walk — to certain destinations. “However, if we consider that people who weigh more than others perceive the world differently, they may in fact be making reasonable behavioral decisions given the way they perceive the environment.”

The researchers recruited 30 women and 36 men from outside a local store, and asked them to stand behind a piece of duct tape that had been placed on the sidewalk. The participants then guessed how far away an orange sports cone was from where they were standing.

After this simple task, the participants filled out a survey about their height, weight, and perceived body size. The researchers also physically measured the participants’ actual height and weight.

The researchers found that a person’s body weight influenced how far away they estimated the cone to be. In particular, those who weighed more tended to perceive the cone as farther away. This was true regardless of whether the participants felt they had a large or small body size. Their beliefs about their own body weight did not influence their distance estimates.

Surprisingly, body mass index (BMI) — a simple measure of body size based on height and weight — was not a factor. “Body weight corresponds to the amount of energetic work that must be done (i.e. the amount of mass that must be transported), whereas BMI corresponds to, in part, the way this weight is distributed,” Sugovica and her colleagues explained.

This finding suggests that a person’s overall body weight, rather than the distribution of fat and muscle, is the critical factor.

“Perception might be influenced by the overall energetic work regardless of the muscle available to help achieve said goal,” the researchers said.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Eye-tracking study suggests that negative comments on social media are more attention-grabbing than positive comments
Cognitive Science

Can you train your brain to unsee optical illusions? Scientists think so

May 12, 2025

A recent study found that radiologists are less susceptible to optical illusions, likely due to their intensive visual training. The research challenges long-standing beliefs that illusions are automatic and suggests perceptual skills can be shaped over time.

Read moreDetails
Diets high in fat and sugar appear to harm cognitive function
Cognitive Science

Diets high in fat and sugar appear to harm cognitive function

May 10, 2025

Consuming a Western-style diet packed with sugar and saturated fats may hurt your brain, not just your waistline. A new study shows poorer performance on spatial memory tasks among people with diets high in processed, unhealthy foods.

Read moreDetails
People with lower cognitive ability more likely to fall for pseudo-profound bullshit
Cognitive Science

People with lower cognitive ability more likely to fall for pseudo-profound bullshit

May 9, 2025

A new meta-analysis published in Applied Cognitive Psychology finds that people with lower cognitive ability are more likely to find meaning in pseudo-profound nonsense. The study identifies key psychological traits linked to susceptibility to feel-good but meaningless statements.

Read moreDetails
Neuroscientists uncover a fascinating fact about social thinking in the brain
Cognitive Science

Neuroscientists uncover a fascinating fact about social thinking in the brain

May 7, 2025

Our brains process social similarity in two ways—by comparing people to each other and by comparing them to ourselves. A new study using brain imaging reveals that these forms of person knowledge are represented in separate areas of the brain.

Read moreDetails
Classical music may promote calmer, more stable fetal heart rhythms, study suggests
Cognitive Science

Classical music may promote calmer, more stable fetal heart rhythms, study suggests

May 6, 2025

Listening to classical music may calm the fetal heartbeat, according to new research. The study found that heart rate patterns became more orderly after music exposure.

Read moreDetails
Women underestimate their spatial intelligence—even when they perform just as well as men
Cognitive Science

Women underestimate their spatial intelligence—even when they perform just as well as men

May 6, 2025

New research shows women underestimate their spatial intelligence, even when they perform just as well as men. This gender gap in self-perception—shaped by personality traits like narcissism and modesty—could help explain why fewer women pursue STEM careers.

Read moreDetails
Scientists create a new color never before seen by human eyes
Cognitive Science

Scientists create a new color never before seen by human eyes

May 5, 2025

A groundbreaking study reveals that humans can experience an entirely new color, thanks to a system that stimulates individual cone cells in the retina. Scientists call the color “olo”—a brilliant blue-green that doesn't occur naturally in human vision.

Read moreDetails
Eye-tracking study: Women’s attention to facial masculinity tied to mating interests and self-perceived attractiveness
Cognitive Science

Negative images hijack attention and linger in memory, new study shows

May 3, 2025

A new study using a specially designed attention task finds that disturbing background images reliably disrupt focus and slow response times. These emotionally negative distractions also heighten negative feelings and are remembered more vividly, suggesting they hijack both attention and memory.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Psychedelics linked to religious disaffiliation—but not spiritual change—in large-scale study

Can you train your brain to unsee optical illusions? Scientists think so

New research links antidepressant effects of escitalopram to endocannabinoid system changes

College students still follow familiar relationship paths despite dating app era, study finds

Women who misrepresent themselves on dating apps more likely to consider cosmetic surgery

A single dose of psilocybin might help reduce symptoms in treatment-resistant depression

Abnormal brain rhythms may offer new insight into Alzheimer’s disease and its link to epilepsy

Left-wing authoritarians are less likely to support physically strong men as leaders

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy