Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

Scientists create a new color never before seen by human eyes

by Eric W. Dolan
May 5, 2025
in Cognitive Science
Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay informed on the latest psychology and neuroscience research—follow PsyPost on LinkedIn for daily updates and insights.

Scientists at the University of California, Berkeley have developed a technology that can directly stimulate individual photoreceptor cells in the human retina, allowing people to perceive colors that have never been seen before. Using a technique they call “Oz,” the scientists created a vivid blue-green hue—described as “unprecedented” in saturation and named “olo”—that lies outside the range of colors normally perceivable by human vision. Their findings, recently published in Science Advances, offer a powerful new tool for studying vision.

The goal of the project was to explore what happens when light is directed not broadly across the eye, but precisely to specific cone cells responsible for color perception. The research builds on earlier work in color theory and retinal imaging. Traditional color display technologies work by mixing red, green, and blue light to stimulate the L (long), M (medium), and S (short) cone cells.

But this always involves some overlap between different types of cones, due to their shared sensitivities. The Oz system attempts something radically different: stimulating just one type of cone—especially the M cone—while avoiding the others entirely. This kind of precise activation is impossible with conventional lighting or screens but becomes possible through finely targeted laser stimulation.

“I teach a visual computing course at UC Berkeley. In preparing for the lectures on color some years ago, I came across Professor Austin Roorda’s work on mapping the cells in the living human retina, and stimulating individual cone cells,” explained Ren Ng, an associate professor in the Department of Electrical Engineering and Computer Sciences.

“From a color theory perspective, it was clear that no light can stimulate only the M cone cells in the retina in normal viewing, so I was filled with curiosity about what it might look like to show someone a square of color where only the M cone cells were stimulated. We teamed up, became friends, and have pursued this research for some years together.”

To conduct the experiment, the researchers first had to map out the retina of each participant in incredible detail. Five subjects, all with normal color vision, underwent adaptive optics scanning and retinal imaging to classify the L, M, and S cones in a small portion of their retina. Then, using an advanced optical system that tracks eye movement and compensates for it in real time, they delivered laser microdoses directly to specific cones within a 0.9° square visual field—roughly the size of a grain of rice held at arm’s length.

This targeting required both high-speed tracking of eye motion and incredibly fine-tuned hardware to adjust the laser beam in real time. Even a slight misfire would stimulate the wrong cone and produce normal colors. But when done accurately, the stimulation of only M cones produced a visual experience beyond what people typically see in nature.

“When I initially wrote to Austin, I guessed that M-only color might look like the ‘greenest green you never saw’ — this is why we called the system to show it Oz Vision, alluding to the brilliant green of the Emerald City in The Wizard of Oz,” Ng told PsyPost.

“But olo looked blue-green. When Oz system performance matured enough (this took years of improvements) to see olo for the first time, the hue surprised me, delighted me, and excited me — because it was such a clear visual change that signified system precision was now close to targeting individual retinal cells at population scale.”

In color matching experiments, participants compared this new color to others and consistently reported that it could not be matched with any natural or artificial light source. They described it as a highly saturated teal or blue-green that could only be approximated by mixing it with white light. This mismatch confirmed that “olo” exists outside the normal human gamut of colors.

The researchers didn’t stop at color patches. They also used the Oz system to display lines and moving dots in image and video form, such as red shapes on an olo background. In these tasks, subjects successfully perceived orientation and motion only when the laser microdoses were delivered accurately. When the targeting was intentionally disrupted, these shapes disappeared or lost their distinct colors, reducing subjects’ performance to guessing. This confirmed that the perceived images were being formed through accurate cell-by-cell stimulation and not from any inherent qualities of the laser itself.

“As we’ve seen in the past few days, the very existence of olo, a hypersaturated teal beyond the human color gamut, has captured the imagination of the public in an enormous way,” Ng said. “That’s natural, because as humans we are very visual animals, and color is right there in the center of our everyday vision — what could feel more natural and complete than our personal sense of color?  I hope what people take away from this story is a sense of the delight that can come from scientific inquiry, of expanding our knowledge, and becoming aware of things just beyond what we can see today.”

The technical achievement of the Oz system lies in its ability to program the retina with unprecedented precision. While other methods—such as silent substitution or adaptation effects—have been used to explore cone-specific activation, they lack the sustained control, spatial resolution, or range that Oz provides. By directly stimulating thousands of cones with rapid pulses of light, the system can produce stable and measurable color experiences, not just fleeting impressions.

The study’s results are significant for both scientific and practical reasons. On the scientific side, they offer a new way to understand the limits and capabilities of human vision. Since all previous color perception research has been bounded by the spectral sensitivities of cone cells, Oz opens a path to explore what the brain does when it receives signals that don’t normally occur in nature. It also allows researchers to test how visual perception is constructed from patterns of cone activation, potentially informing models of visual processing at the neural level.

On the practical side, the Oz platform may have future applications in vision science, clinical diagnostics, or even entertainment. For example, it could be used to test or train individuals with color deficiencies by simulating the presence of a missing photopigment. It might even allow for the perception of “imaginary” colors in people who normally experience the world in only two or three dimensions of color. With further refinement, Oz could potentially enhance virtual or augmented reality experiences by expanding the palette of perceivable colors beyond what any display can currently show.

There are limitations to the current system. It only works in a small region of the retina and requires the subject to maintain strict gaze fixation. Expanding the technique to allow free viewing or to stimulate a wider field of vision would require major technological advances in both optics and computing. The method is also limited to laboratory conditions, relying on equipment that is currently too complex and expensive for broader use.

“This is a basic science project, not a technology or product development effort,” Ng noted. “Folks must understand that they will not be seeing olo on their smartphones or televisions anytime soon. Also, Oz Vision is currently a very small display — the size of your thumbnail at arm’s length or 5 times the area of the full moon.”

Still, the proof-of-principle achieved in this study is an important step forward. By showing that it is possible to program the retina at the level of individual cells and evoke perceptual experiences that go beyond natural vision, the researchers have introduced a new class of display technology—one based not on pixels, but on the biology of the eye itself.

“Olo is a visual sign that Oz Vision is now ready for broad scientific work — we can programmably set the activation of thousands of photoreceptors in the retina, as a basic platform for vision science and neuroscience,” Ng explained. “Some immediate goals are to simulate and study the visual perception that occurs in diseases where a large fraction of the cells have died off but patients are visually unaware; and to study the boosting of color dimension, such as boosting color blindness to full color vision, or full color to tetrachromacy.”

“But the long-term question is the scientific mystery underlying perception. Retinal signals differ so markedly from our experience of color vision, and it remains a mystery how the brain makes sense of such signals and produces the color vision that we enjoy every waking moment. The Oz Vision system enables us to scientifically probe this perceptual miracle in unprecedented ways, and hopefully to elucidate the underlying neural mechanisms.”

“I want to spotlight James Fong and Hannah Doyle, the graduate researchers who co-led this work to success over many years, requiring enormous talent and perseverance,” Ng added. “And my good friend and collaborator, Austin Roorda, who has worked for decades on the science and technology of looking into the eye, seeing individual cells in the retina, and stimulating them with mind-boggling precision.”

The study, “Novel color via stimulation of individual photoreceptors at population scale,” was authored by James Fong, Hannah K. Doyle, Congli Wang, Alexandra E. Boehm, Sofie R. Herbeck, Vimal Prabhu Pandiyan, Brian P. Schmidt, Pavan Tiruveedhula, John E. Vanston, William S. Tuten, Ramkumar Sabesan, Austin Roorda, and Ren Ng.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Even in healthy adults, high blood sugar levels are linked to impaired brain function
Memory

Neuroscientists decode how people juggle multiple items in working memory

July 8, 2025

New neuroscience research shows how the brain decides which memories deserve more attention. By tracking brain activity, scientists found that the frontal cortex helps direct limited memory resources, allowing people to remember high-priority information more precisely than less relevant details.

Read moreDetails
New study uncovers a surprising effect of cold-water immersion
Cognitive Science

New study uncovers a surprising effect of cold-water immersion

July 8, 2025

Cold-water immersion increases energy expenditure—but it may also drive people to eat more afterward. A study in Physiology & Behavior found participants consumed significantly more food following cold exposure, possibly due to internal cooling effects that continue after leaving the water.

Read moreDetails
Positive attitudes toward AI linked to more prone to problematic social media use
Cognitive Science

People with higher cognitive ability have weaker moral foundations, new study finds

July 7, 2025

A large study has found that individuals with greater cognitive ability are less likely to endorse moral values such as compassion, fairness, loyalty, and purity. The results point to a consistent negative relationship between intelligence and moral intuitions.

Read moreDetails
These common sounds can impair your learning, according to new psychology research
Cognitive Science

These common sounds can impair your learning, according to new psychology research

July 4, 2025

Your brain’s ancient defense system might be sabotaging your test scores. New research suggests our "behavioral immune system," which makes us subconsciously alert to signs of illness, can be triggered by coughs and sniffles.

Read moreDetails
From fireflies to brain cells: Unraveling the complex web of synchrony in networks
Addiction

Understanding “neuronal ensembles” could revolutionize addiction treatment

July 3, 2025

The same brain system that rewards you for a delicious meal is hijacked by drugs like fentanyl. A behavioral neuroscientist explains how understanding the specific memories behind these rewards is the key to treating addiction without harming our essential survival instincts.

Read moreDetails
Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time
Memory

Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time

July 3, 2025

Our perception of time is more fragile than we think. Scientists have uncovered a powerful illusion where repeated exposure to information makes us misremember it as happening much further in the past, significantly distorting our mental timelines.

Read moreDetails
Peppermint tea boosts memory and attention—but why?
Cognitive Science

Peppermint tea boosts memory and attention—but why?

July 2, 2025

Can a cup of peppermint tea sharpen your mind? A new study suggests it can—but not in the way scientists expected. Improved memory and attention followed the tea, but increased brain blood flow wasn't the reason why.

Read moreDetails
Scientists reveal ChatGPT’s left-wing bias — and how to “jailbreak” it
Artificial Intelligence

ChatGPT and “cognitive debt”: New study suggests AI might be hurting your brain’s ability to think

July 1, 2025

Researchers at MIT investigated how writing with ChatGPT affects brain activity and recall. Their findings indicate that reliance on AI may lead to reduced mental engagement, prompting concerns about cognitive “offloading” and its implications for education.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Neuroscientists decode how people juggle multiple items in working memory

Inside the bored brain: Unlocking the power of the default mode network

Choline imbalance in the brain linked to with cognitive symptoms in young depression patients

Scientists who relocate more often start Nobel research up to two years earlier

Sedentary time linked to faster brain aging in older adults, study finds

People with short-video addiction show altered brain responses during decision-making

New study uncovers a surprising effect of cold-water immersion

Being adopted doesn’t change how teens handle love and dating

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy