Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Developmental Psychology

Adults and children judge coincidences differently, study reveals

by Mane Kara-Yakoubian
August 15, 2024
in Developmental Psychology
(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

Coincidences—unexpected confluences of events that seemingly defy probability—captivate people across cultures and ages. New research published in Cognition sheds light on the cognitive mechanisms underpinning the recognition of coincidences and how these mechanisms evolve as we grow older.

The cognitive mechanisms involved in representing coincidences are thought to be part of broader causal reasoning processes. Research suggests that even preschool-aged children demonstrate flexible causal reasoning abilities, adjusting their inferences based on how data is generated and exploring more when causal relationships are ambiguous. Despite these early-emerging capacities, it remains unclear whether young children can distinguish between mere coincidences and causally linked events, as adults do.

Experiment 1 involved 117 English-speaking adults aged 18 to 74 recruited through Amazon Mechanical Turk and Prolific. Participants read eight short vignettes, each describing a set of co-occurring events. The vignettes were designed to either include or exclude information that could potentially explain the co-occurrences.

The number of co-occurring events also varied across vignettes, with scenarios involving two, three, a few, or all events co-occurring. For each vignette, participants rated how surprising they found the events (on a 4-point scale), whether they considered the events to be a coincidence and whether they believed there was a reason for the co-occurrence (on binary scales). Participants’ demographic information and responses to additional exploratory questions about belief in supernatural powers, religious practices, and frequency of noticing coincidences were also collected.

Qiong Cao and Lisa Feigenson found that judgments of coincidences were influenced by the presence of explanations and the number of co-occurring events. Participants were more likely to judge events as coincidences when no explanations were provided. When explanations were present, they were more likely to believe there was a reason for the events’ co-occurrence and rated the events as less surprising. When no explanation was available, coincidence ratings initially increased with the number of co-occurring events but then decreased when all events co-occurred, suggesting that too many co-occurrences prompted them to infer an underlying causal explanation.

Experiment 2 involved 94 children aged 4 to 10 years, tested in a university child development laboratory or a local science museum. The vignettes used in Study 1 were adapted for children, with the number of vignettes reduced to four to maintain attention. The vignettes varied in the presence of potential explanations and the number of co-occurring events, similar to the adult study. They were read aloud by an experimenter, accompanied by simple illustrations to help children understand the events. After each vignette, children rated how surprising they found the events using a pictorial scale featuring cartoon faces and answered whether they thought the events were a coincidence and if there was a reason for the co-occurrence.

The results indicated that even the youngest children were sensitive to the presence of explanations, showing a basic understanding of coincidences. When no explanations were provided, children were more likely to judge the events as coincidences, believe there was no reason for the events’ co-occurrence, and find the events more surprising.

However, only older children (7 to 10 years old) showed sensitivity to the number of co-occurring events similar to adults. Older children’s coincidence ratings initially increased with the number of co-occurring events but decreased when all events co-occurred, whereas younger children (4 to 7 years old) did not show this sensitivity.

These findings suggest that the cognitive mechanisms underlying coincidence recognition are present early in life but become more refined with age and experience.

One limitation noted by the authors is the reliance on verbal explanations and judgments, which might have influenced younger children’s responses due to their developing language skills.

The research, “Children’s Representation of Coincidence”, was authored by Qiong Cao and Lisa Feigenson.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Mothers’ depressive symptoms appear to affect children’s socio-emotional development, partly through parenting practices
Developmental Psychology

Mother’s childhood trauma linked to emotional and behavioral issues in her children, study finds

May 18, 2025

A recent study suggests that childhood trauma doesn’t end with the individual—it can influence the next generation. Maternal adversity was linked to children’s conduct, emotions, and cognition through economic strain, depression, relationship conflict, and parenting behavior.

Read moreDetails
Childhood adversity linked to fear overgeneralization and reduced safety learning in teens
Anxiety

Childhood adversity linked to fear overgeneralization and reduced safety learning in teens

May 16, 2025

Research on adolescents exposed to early trauma reveals impaired fear learning: those with childhood adversity showed less ability to distinguish safety from threat and were more prone to overgeneralize fear, highlighting a possible pathway to future mental health problems.

Read moreDetails
Maternal warmth in childhood predicts key personality traits years later
Developmental Psychology

Maternal warmth in childhood predicts key personality traits years later

May 11, 2025

A new study of British twins suggests that children who receive more maternal affection grow into more open, conscientious, and agreeable adults. The findings point to lasting effects of parenting, even after accounting for genetics and shared family environment.

Read moreDetails
Parental warmth—not poverty or danger—predicts positive world beliefs in adulthood
Developmental Psychology

Parental warmth—not poverty or danger—predicts positive world beliefs in adulthood

May 10, 2025

Groundbreaking research spanning 14 years and eight countries reveals that warm parenting during childhood strongly predicts young adults’ beliefs that the world is good, safe, and enticing—while material hardship and harsh discipline showed little effect on worldview.

Read moreDetails
Handwriting trumps visual learning for teaching English to children
Cognitive Science

New study shows handwriting boosts early reading skills more than typing

May 1, 2025

New research suggests that the shift from handwriting to digital tools in early education may come at a cost. In an experiment with 5-year-olds, those who practiced writing by hand showed better letter naming, spelling, and word reading than those who used keyboards.

Read moreDetails
Neural responses to mistakes may help explain how depression risk is passed from mothers to daughters
Depression

Neural responses to mistakes may help explain how depression risk is passed from mothers to daughters

May 1, 2025

A new study suggests that the way the brain responds to mistakes could help explain how depression is passed from mothers to daughters. Researchers found that certain neural signals related to error processing were altered in mothers with depression and were linked to similar patterns in their daughters.

Read moreDetails
Friendships across gender and race linked to greater kindness in children, study finds
Developmental Psychology

Friendships across gender and race linked to greater kindness in children, study finds

April 29, 2025

New research suggests that diverse childhood friendships—especially those across gender—can encourage kindness not just within friendships, but toward peers from other social groups.

Read moreDetails
Infrared imaging uncovers emotional sensitivity in 10-month-old babies
Developmental Psychology

Infrared imaging uncovers emotional sensitivity in 10-month-old babies

April 28, 2025

A new cross-cultural study reveals that by 10 months of age, babies show both physiological and behavioral signs of emotional contagion.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Where you flirt matters: New research shows setting shapes romantic success

Psychedelic’s anti-anxiety effects can be separated from hallucinations by targeting specific brain circuits

New research reveals aging shifts gender stereotypes in unexpected ways

Optimistic individuals are more likely to respond to SSRI antidepressants

Brain oscillations reveal dynamic shifts in creative thought during metaphor generation

Surprisingly widespread brain activity supports economic decision-making, new study finds

Scientists finds altered attention-related brain connectivity in youth with anxiety

From fixed pulses to smart stimulation: Parkinson’s treatment takes a leap forward

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy