Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

Cognitive distancing appears to enhance decision-making performance, new study reveals

by Eric W. Dolan
August 25, 2023
in Cognitive Science, Mental Health
(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay informed on the latest psychology and neuroscience research—follow PsyPost on LinkedIn for daily updates and insights.

New research published in Psychological Medicine provides evidence that a common psychotherapeutic strategy can enhance performance during a reinforcement learning task. The study indicates that cognitive distancing improves decision-making by making people more deliberate in their choices and heightening their sensitivity to adverse results. These changes in how people learn from outcomes might explain why cognitive distancing is helpful in managing emotions.

Emotion regulation difficulties are prevalent across psychiatric disorders, and effective psychological treatments have been shown to improve these difficulties. Cognitive distancing, where individuals distance themselves from negative thoughts and emotions, is a fundamental technique in cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) and other therapies. It aims to promote disengagement from intense emotions and reduce distress.

In essence, cognitive distancing involves mentally stepping back from immediate reactions and viewing situations from a more objective and calm perspective. By adopting the viewpoint of an external observer, individuals can gain psychological distance from their intense emotions and negative thoughts.

In contrast to the advancements in understanding the pharmacological mechanisms of psychiatric treatments, the mechanisms underlying psychological interventions remain unclear. Since reward learning is impaired in many psychiatric disorders and is targeted by both pharmacological and psychological treatments, the researchers aimed to investigate whether cognitive distancing could impact reward learning in a manner similar to pharmacological interventions.

“The mechanisms of current treatments for mental ill-health – both how they work, and why, for many, they don’t work – remain poorly understood. This is especially true for psychological therapies such as cognitive behavioural therapy,” explained study author Quentin Dercon, a PhD student at University College London.

“However, it has long been thought that pharmacological (drug-based) treatments such as antidepressants may act through their effects on neurotransmitter systems to resolve biases in learning and decision-making, which are known to emerge in conditions such as depression. We were interested in testing whether a specific technique taught in many modalities of psychological therapy – ‘cognitive distancing’ – may also acutely affect simple learning processes, as this could help to explain how emotion regulation strategies such as distancing may help to resolve symptoms of mental ill-health.”

To test this hypothesis, the researchers recruited a diverse group of participants through the Prolific platform. This online study comprised a reinforcement learning task and a series of psychiatric questionnaires. Out of the 995 participants, half received a cognitive distancing intervention, whereas the other half did not.

Participants were divided into two groups: one received a cognitive distancing intervention, while the other did not. The reinforcement learning task involved choosing between pairs of Japanese Hiragana characters associated with different reward probabilities.

The researchers found that participants who underwent the cognitive distancing intervention performed better on the reinforcement learning task. They exhibited improved accuracy in choosing symbols associated with different reward probabilities, particularly in pairs with closer reward probabilities. This suggested that cognitive distancing might have enhanced participants’ ability to learn from feedback and make more advantageous choices.

“We found that teaching people a technique borrowed from psychological therapy was sufficient to induce subtle changes in how they learned the value of choices while playing a simple game, and improved their overall performance compared to control participants,” Dercon told PsyPost. “This was despite the fact that this intervention – known as ‘cognitive distancing’ – was subtle and self-guided: participants were simply told to try to control their immediate emotional response to feedback (i.e., being told whether they were ‘correct’ or ‘incorrect’) after making choices.

“Our work provides some preliminary evidence that techniques taught in psychological therapy may help improve symptoms of mental ill-health by encouraging people to change how they view and learn from information in the world, processes which are known to be biased in mental health disorders such as depression and anxiety.”

Importantly, the study revealed that cognitive distancing led to alterations in reinforcement learning processes. Distanced participants exhibited higher inverse temperature values, indicating a decreased level of stochasticity in their choices. In other words, they were more likely to consistently choose the option that had a higher expected reward, rather than making more exploratory or random choices. Distanced participants also demonstrated increased sensitivity to negative feedback.

“We found evidence that, compared to control participants, participants practicing cognitive distancing had higher negative learning rates, which means that they more quickly replaced old information about the value of certain choices with new information when that information was negative (i.e., being told that choice was ‘incorrect’),” Dercon explained.

“This is counterintuitive, given we would expect biases towards negative information to be associated with poorer mental health. However, in the context of our experiment, this was not associated with worse performance – in fact, those in the distancing group on average performed slightly better in the task.”

“In exploratory analyses, we found some clues as to why this may be the case – this bias (relative to control participants) appeared to emerge later in the task, when some learning had taken place, and so negative information may be rarer but also potentially more informative. This is why we speculate that this may represent more effective engagement with negative information.”

Previous studies have found that the use of antidepressant medications can also lead to heightened sensitivity to negative outcomes. This indicates that psychological interventions like cognitive distancing and pharmaceutical treatments such as antidepressants might share a common mechanism.

While the new study provides important insights into the effects of cognitive distancing on decision-making, the research has a few limitations to consider. Dercon highlighted three significant caveats. Firstly, the participants recruited for the study represented the general population, encompassing a variety of psychiatric symptoms. Therefore, the conclusions drawn from this research might not directly extend to clinical populations that specifically receive psychological interventions.

Secondly, “this is an acute intervention (and a subtle one at that) – it cannot be seen as analogous to the effects of long-term psychological therapy.” Lastly, the researchers were unable to assess how effectively individuals were applying cognitive distancing during the study. Consequently, substantial variations likely existed among participants within the group practicing cognitive distancing.

These limitations underscore the need for further research to unravel the interactions between cognitive interventions, psychological processes, and mental health outcomes, Dercon explained: “Do psychological interventions such as distancing modulate learning processes similarly in those with high levels of (e.g., depression) symptoms? Do changes in learning processes mediate the positive effects of psychotherapeutic techniques such as distancing longitudinally? What factors predict how well an individual can engage with and benefit from psychotherapeutic techniques such as distancing, and psychological therapies more broadly?”

The study, “A core component of psychological therapy causes adaptive changes in computational learning mechanisms“, was authored by Quentin Dercon, Sara Z. Mehrhof, Timothy R. Sandhu, Caitlin Hitchcock, Rebecca P. Lawson, Diego A. Pizzagalli, Tim Dalgleish, and Camilla L. Nord.

TweetSendScanShareSendPin31ShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Researchers identify neural mechanism behind memory prioritization
Memory

Researchers identify neural mechanism behind memory prioritization

June 30, 2025

A new brain imaging study shows that when people try to remember multiple things, their brains give more precise attention to the most important item. The frontal cortex helps allocate memory resources, boosting accuracy for high-priority information.

Read moreDetails
Study explores psychological pathways from attachment style to love addiction
Addiction

Love addiction linked to memory and attention problems

June 30, 2025

Obsessive romantic attachment may be more than an emotional burden—it could also impair your thinking. A new study reveals that love addiction, especially when fueled by anxiety and social media use, is linked to memory problems and daily cognitive failures.

Read moreDetails
Positive early experiences may buffer suicidal thoughts in those with trauma symptoms, new study finds
Early Life Adversity and Childhood Maltreatment

Positive early experiences may buffer suicidal thoughts in those with trauma symptoms, new study finds

June 29, 2025

Positive relationships in childhood may play a lasting role in protecting college students from suicidal ideation tied to trauma, a new study finds, offering promising implications for both clinical care and campus mental health programs.

Read moreDetails
MIND diet linked to better attentional control in schoolchildren, study finds
ADHD

MIND diet linked to better attentional control in schoolchildren, study finds

June 29, 2025

Children who ate diets more closely aligned with the MIND diet performed better on a task measuring attentional control, according to a new study. The effect was not observed for children who simply followed U.S. dietary guidelines.

Read moreDetails
Dark traits predict social appearance anxiety, study finds
Body Image and Body Dysmorphia

Digital therapy cuts body image anxiety in men by tackling appearance-related safety behaviors

June 29, 2025

Researchers have found that appearance-related habits—like repeatedly checking mirrors or asking for reassurance—maintain anxiety in men. A month-long digital intervention helped participants reduce these behaviors, leading to improvements in body image, social anxiety, and depression.

Read moreDetails
Scientists show how you’re unknowingly sealing yourself in an information bubble
Cognitive Science

Scientists show how you’re unknowingly sealing yourself in an information bubble

June 29, 2025

Scientists have found that belief polarization doesn’t always come from misinformation or social media bubbles. Instead, it often begins with a simple search. Our choice of words—and the algorithm’s response—can subtly seal us inside our own informational comfort zones.

Read moreDetails
Liver health may influence mental health via inflammation and glutamate levels
Anxiety

Liver health may influence mental health via inflammation and glutamate levels

June 28, 2025

A new study suggests that diets high in fat and fructose can damage the liver and trigger anxiety-like behaviors in mice. The research also found that corilagin, a natural compound, reversed many of these harmful effects.

Read moreDetails
Muscle contractions release chemical signals that promote brain network development
Memory

Sleep helps stitch memories into cognitive maps, according to new neuroscience breakthrough

June 28, 2025

Scientists have discovered that forming a mental map of a new environment takes more than just recognizing individual places—it also requires sleep. The study highlights how weakly tuned neurons gradually become synchronized to encode space as a connected whole.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

People who are more likely to die seem to care less about the future

Researchers identify neural mechanism behind memory prioritization

Love addiction linked to memory and attention problems

Positive early experiences may buffer suicidal thoughts in those with trauma symptoms, new study finds

Readers struggle to understand AI’s role in news writing, study suggests

MIND diet linked to better attentional control in schoolchildren, study finds

Digital therapy cuts body image anxiety in men by tackling appearance-related safety behaviors

Scientists show how you’re unknowingly sealing yourself in an information bubble

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy