Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology Political Psychology Authoritarianism

Epistemic mistrust and dogmatism predict preference for authoritarian-looking leaders

by Eric W. Dolan
June 20, 2025
in Authoritarianism
[Adobe Stock]

[Adobe Stock]

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay informed on the latest psychology and neuroscience research—follow PsyPost on LinkedIn for daily updates and insights.

A new study published in the journal Politics and the Life Sciences sheds light on how the way people come to trust or mistrust information can influence their political views and the kinds of leaders they find appealing. Although a person’s general trust in communicated knowledge—what psychologists call “epistemic trust”—does not directly predict whom they would vote for, it is linked to political ideology and dogmatic thinking, which are themselves related to preferences for authoritarian-looking leaders.

“In recent years, we’ve seen a troubling decline in trust toward major institutions like political leaders, the media, and science. This loss of trust has serious consequences, as shown during the COVID-19 pandemic, when people’s political beliefs often influenced whether they followed scientific advice,” said study author Mariana von Mohr, a researcher at Royal Holloway, University of London, and at the Centre for the Politics of Feelings.

“We were interested in how people’s ability to trust information from others—something called epistemic trust, which develops early in life—might shape the kind of political leaders they prefer. Specifically, we wanted to understand whether lower epistemic trust makes people more resistant to different viewpoints and more drawn to strong, dominant leaders who offer certainty and clear-cut answers.”

To investigate this, the researchers recruited 1,096 participants, with roughly equal numbers from the United Kingdom and the United States. The participants were selected to be representative of their national populations in terms of age, sex, and ethnicity. They completed several online tasks and questionnaires.

One of the primary tools in the study was the Epistemic Trust, Mistrust, and Credulity Questionnaire. This measure captures three different ways people relate to communicated information: epistemic trust (the belief that others provide useful, relevant, and reliable knowledge), epistemic mistrust (a default suspicion toward information from others), and epistemic credulity (an overly accepting stance, regardless of source reliability). These traits are thought to stem from early social experiences, such as attachment relationships with caregivers.

The questionnaire includes statements that reflect these three stances toward information. For example, a person with high epistemic trust might agree with the statement, “I usually ask people for advice when I have a personal problem,” showing a willingness to rely on others for guidance. Someone with high epistemic mistrust might relate more to, “I’d prefer to find things out for myself on the internet rather than asking people for information,” reflecting a tendency to avoid relying on others. Meanwhile, a person high in epistemic credulity might endorse, “I am often considered naïve because I believe almost anything that people tell me,” suggesting a lack of discrimination in evaluating information sources.

To assess political preferences in a more behavior-based way, participants completed a leader choice task. On each trial, they saw a pair of computer-generated, gender-neutral faces that varied in perceived dominance and trustworthiness. These facial traits were chosen because they are thought to trigger deep-rooted social evaluations. Participants were asked which person they would prefer to vote for, allowing the researchers to measure each individual’s likelihood of choosing leaders with more authoritarian features (dominant and less trustworthy faces).

The researchers then analyzed the data using two statistical approaches. The first was a structural equation model, which allowed them to test a theory about how different psychological traits might be linked. The second was a network analysis, which explored the relationships between all the measured variables without imposing any assumed structure.

The results of both analyses showed that epistemic trust did not directly predict preferences for authoritarian-looking leaders. However, people who scored higher on epistemic trust tended to be more politically liberal and less dogmatic in their thinking. In contrast, people who were more epistemically mistrustful or credulous tended to be more dogmatic. Dogmatism, in turn, was associated with more conservative ideology and a greater preference for authoritarian-looking leaders.

In other words, epistemic trust influenced political choices indirectly, through its connection to political ideology and dogmatic thinking. The findings support the idea that our willingness or unwillingness to accept communicated knowledge—shaped early in life—can influence the cognitive styles and belief systems we carry into adulthood, including how rigid or flexible we are in our thinking and how we relate to political leaders.

One of the most striking findings from the network analysis was that epistemic trust, political ideology, and leader choice were not directly connected to one another except through their shared relationship with dogmatism. Dogmatism appeared as a central node in the network, acting as a bridge between trust-related traits and political behavior. This suggests that rigid or closed-minded thinking may be a key mechanism linking developmental dispositions, such as epistemic mistrust, to political preferences.

“Our research shows that when people struggle to trust the knowledge and intentions of others, they tend to become more dogmatic, holding tightly to their beliefs even when faced with evidence that challenges them,” von Mohr told PsyPost. “This kind of thinking can make strong, uncompromising political leaders more appealing.”

“It’s important for people to realize that their ability to trust others affects how open they are to different perspectives, which in turn shapes their political choices. Creating early social environments that encourage trust and openness to new information may help people stay more flexible in their thinking and make more thoughtful political decisions.”

The researchers also found some differences between the U.K. and U.S. samples. In the U.S., epistemic mistrust and credulity were linked more strongly to political trust, and political trust was positively associated with political ideology—meaning that liberals were more likely to trust politicians. In the U.K., by contrast, political trust was lower among liberals, likely reflecting dissatisfaction with the conservative government in power at the time. These differences underscore how national political context can shape the expression of more general psychological traits.

But the study, like all research, includes some caveats.

“One important limitation is that our study is based on correlations, meaning we can’t say for sure that one thing causes another,” von Mohr noted. “We found links between low trust, dogmatic thinking, and support for certain political leaders, but we can’t prove the direction of these relationships. That said, we looked at them in an order that makes sense based on what we know from psychology: epistemic trust tends to form early in life, while dogmatism and political beliefs usually develop later. So although we can’t make strong claims about cause and effect, our interpretation fits with how these traits are generally understood to unfold over time.”

Despite these limitations, the study offers valuable insights into the psychological roots of political behavior. It suggests that developmental factors that shape our general trust in communicated knowledge are related to the way we think about politics, how dogmatic or open we are in our beliefs, and even the kinds of leaders we are drawn to. In particular, it highlights dogmatism as a potential pathway through which early dispositions may lead to political preferences, including a tendency to support authoritarian leaders.

“One important takeaway is that epistemic trust, while shaped early in life, isn’t set in stone,” von Mohr added. “People can become more open to others’ perspectives over time, especially if they’re in environments that support respectful dialogue and critical thinking. This is especially relevant today, when mistrust and political polarisation seem to be growing. We hope our study encourages further work on how to rebuild trust in ways that support more open-minded and engaged communities.”

The study, “A leader I can(not) trust: Understanding the path from epistemic trust to political leader choices via dogmatism,” was authored by Mariana von Mohr, Kobi Hackenburg, Michal Tanzer, Aikaterini Fotopoulou, Chloe Campbell, and Manos Tsakiris.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Fear predicts authoritarian attitudes across cultures, with conservatives most affected
Authoritarianism

Fear predicts authoritarian attitudes across cultures, with conservatives most affected

June 13, 2025

A sweeping study of over 84,000 people across 59 countries found that individuals who feel threatened by crime, poverty, or instability are more likely to support authoritarian governance—especially in Western nations and among politically right-leaning individuals.

Read moreDetails
Political ambivalence has a surprising relationship with support for violence
Authoritarianism

Perceived social breakdown fuels desire for authoritarian leaders, new psychology study shows

June 10, 2025

New research provides causal evidence that perceived societal breakdown—known as anomie—can increase support for authoritarianism by eroding feelings of control and increasing uncertainty.

Read moreDetails
Your brain’s insulation might become emergency energy during a marathon
Authoritarianism

Major study points to evolved psychology behind support for strongmen

June 5, 2025

A new cross-cultural study finds that people across 25 countries are more likely to support dominant, authoritarian leaders when facing intergroup conflict. The results suggest that humans may have evolved psychological instincts that favor forceful leadership during times of threat.

Read moreDetails
Donald Trump’s presidency associated with significant changes in the topography of prejudice in the United States
Authoritarianism

Authoritarian beliefs predict whether voters see Trump or Clinton as psychopathic

June 4, 2025

Researchers found that voters’ authoritarian tendencies influenced how they judged the psychopathic traits of 2016 presidential candidates. Those high in authoritarianism were more likely to view Trump favorably and Clinton as psychologically disordered—and vice versa.

Read moreDetails
Authoritarianism in parents may hinder a key cognitive skill in their children
Authoritarianism

Authoritarianism in parents may hinder a key cognitive skill in their children

June 2, 2025

A new study suggests that mothers who favor social hierarchies and obedience to authority use less perspective-taking language with their children—especially when discussing people from different ethnic backgrounds. Their children also show weaker ability to understand others’ thoughts and feelings.

Read moreDetails
A single Trump tweet has been connected to a rise in arrests of white Americans
Authoritarianism

New study helps explain rising Trump support among minority voters

May 29, 2025

The belief that only conservatives prefer authoritarian leaders is upended by new research showing ethnic minorities—regardless of political affiliation—are more supportive of strong leadership than White liberals. The study suggests generalized trust is a key psychological factor.

Read moreDetails
Left-wing authoritarians are less likely to support physically strong men as leaders
Authoritarianism

Left-wing authoritarians are less likely to support physically strong men as leaders

May 12, 2025

Do muscles make a man a better leader? That depends on your politics. A new study finds conservatives are drawn to strong men in leadership roles, while left-wing authoritarians are more likely to shy away from physical dominance.

Read moreDetails
New study uncovers links between dark triad personality traits and disordered eating habits
Authoritarianism

Dark personalities more prevalent in less democratic countries, massive study shows

May 3, 2025

A massive global study finds that people living in democratic nations are less likely to score high on the “dark triad” of Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy—and more likely to be empathetic and satisfied with life.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Scientists shed light on how forgiveness does and doesn’t reshape memories

Epistemic mistrust and dogmatism predict preference for authoritarian-looking leaders

Rare post-orgasm illness causes days-long flu-like symptoms, but research into its cause remains limited

Gut bacteria may play a causal role in obsessive-compulsive disorder, study suggests

Regular cannabis use linked to changes in brain activity regulating movement

Do AI tools undermine our sense of creativity? New study says yes

Simple social rituals like eye contact and small talk are psychologically powerful

New research links heatwaves to depression and anxiety in adolescents, with some at higher risk

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy