Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

Fascinating new psychology research shows how music shapes imagination

by Eric W. Dolan
August 27, 2025
in Cognitive Science, Music
Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay informed on the latest psychology and neuroscience research—follow PsyPost on LinkedIn for daily updates and insights.

During the COVID-19 pandemic, many people reported turning to music not just for entertainment, but for comfort, support, and even companionship. Now, a new study published in Scientific Reports provides evidence that this sense of “music as company” may be more than a metaphor. Researchers found that music listening can shape mental imagery by increasing the presence of social themes in people’s imagined scenes.

The idea that music offers social comfort has been widely reported in surveys and interviews, especially during periods of isolation such as pandemic lockdowns. Listeners often say they use music “to keep them company” or to ease feelings of loneliness. But the extent to which music genuinely prompts social thinking—rather than simply modulating mood—has been unclear. Most prior research has focused on how music affects memory, emotion, or passive mind-wandering. Few studies have examined how music shapes the content of intentional mental imagery, particularly whether it elicits social scenes or interactions.

This distinction is important because directed mental imagery is used in various clinical and therapeutic settings. Techniques such as imagery rescripting or exposure therapy rely on a person’s ability to vividly imagine scenarios. If music can reliably shift the content of such imagery toward social themes, it might offer new ways to enhance therapeutic outcomes or support individuals struggling with loneliness.

“There have been many reports of people listening to music to ‘keep them company,'” said study author Steffen A. Herff, a Horizon Fellow and leader of the Sydney, Music, Mind, and Body lab at the Sydney Conservatorium of Music at the University of Sydney. “The number of these reports was particularly high during the pandemic isolation periods. But whether this is just a figure of speech, or an actual empirically observable effect of music on social thought was previously unclear, despite its great applicational implications.”

To explore this, the researchers designed two experiments involving over 600 participants. In the first experiment, participants were asked to perform a directed imagery task. They watched a brief video clip showing a solitary figure beginning a journey toward a distant mountain, and were then instructed to close their eyes and imagine how the journey continued. During this 90-second imagination phase, they either heard no sound or listened to folk music in Spanish, Italian, or Swedish.

Stay informed with the latest psychology and neuroscience research—sign up for PsyPost’s newsletter and get new discoveries delivered straight to your inbox.

Each song was played in both vocal and instrumental versions, and participants were either fluent or non-fluent in the language of the lyrics. This allowed the researchers to test whether comprehension or vocal presence mattered for the effect. Across the three language groups, 600 participants took part, split evenly between native and non-native speakers of each language.

After each imagination trial, participants described what they imagined and rated aspects such as vividness and emotional tone. These descriptions were then analyzed using a topic modeling technique called Latent Dirichlet Allocation, which allowed the researchers to identify recurring themes across participants’ narratives.

The researchers found strong evidence that music had an impact on the characteristics of mental imagery. Compared to silence, music consistently led to more vivid mental scenes, more positive emotional tone, and greater perceived time and distance traveled in the imagined journey.

More notably, music also increased the presence of social themes. One of the nine identified topics—labeled Topic I—was clearly centered around social interaction, including words like “people,” “friend,” “village,” and “together.” This topic appeared far more frequently in participants’ imagery when they were listening to music. The effect held across nearly all music conditions tested (30 out of 36), suggesting a consistent influence of music on the presence of social content in imagined scenes.

“The effect of induced social interactions into imagination was much stronger than we originally anticipated,” Herff told PsyPost. “The probability of imagination to contain social interactions in our experiment is more than three times higher when participants listen to music, compared to silence.”

This shift toward social imagery occurred regardless of whether the song included lyrics or whether the listener understood the language. The effect also remained consistent when the vocals were removed, suggesting that the presence of a human voice or semantic content was not necessary for the social effect to emerge.

“Music’s ability to increase social imagination works even if you don’t understand the lyrics of the song, for example because it is in a different language,” Herff said. “In fact, it even works if there are no lyrics at all! Together, this tells us that it’s not simply a question of hearing the human voice that is driving this.”

One exception occurred with an Italian folk song describing a communal grape harvest, where understanding the lyrics amplified the effect—highlighting how specific lyrical content can enhance music’s social influence under certain conditions.

In a second experiment, the researchers used a stable diffusion model to generate images based on participants’ written descriptions of their mental imagery. These visualizations allowed for a more intuitive grasp of the differences between imagery during music and silence.

A new group of 60 participants then viewed pairs of these images—one generated from a music condition, the other from silence—and tried to guess which image was imagined while listening to music. Half of these participants completed the task in silence, while the other half listened to the same music that the original participant had heard.

Those who listened to music during the task performed better, suggesting that music provides contextual cues that help people interpret others’ imagined content. In effect, they were better able to recognize the emotional or thematic signature of music-influenced imagery when they were themselves immersed in the same auditory context.

“Interestingly, when a new group of participants was provided with representations of what the initial participants imagined during silence and during music, they could tell which content was previously imagined during music listening, and which was imagined during silence, but only if the new participants also listened to the music,” Herff told PsyPost. “This tells us that there is a ’theory of mind’ when it comes to music-evoked mental imagery. In other words, you can imagine what someone else might imagine when listening to music.”

To further validate the results, a research assistant unaware of the study’s design manually annotated all 4,200 participant responses for signs of social interaction, temperature, brightness, and narrative perspective.

This analysis showed that descriptions written after music trials were more likely to include social elements and described warmer, brighter environments. In contrast, descriptions following silence tended to be darker, colder, and lonelier. About 39% of music condition descriptions included some form of social interaction, compared to just 12% in the silent condition.

These findings add another layer of evidence that music listening can facilitate social thought, even when people are engaging in a solitary and abstract task like mental imagery.

“Mental imagery is a notoriously difficult phenomenon to study due to its elusive and deeply personal nature,” Herff said. “With our directed mental imagery paradigm, we can now collect data on what people imagine with enough constraint (i.e., directed imagined journey from a clearly defined starting point towards a clearly defined topographical landmark) to do comparisons across conditions (e.g., music vs silence) but also enough freedom (i.e., anything could happen during these imagined journeys) for people to truly unfold their imagination.”

“I believe our findings provide support for an intuition about imagination, music, and their interaction, that many who explore the topic already have, no matter if they approach it from an empirical, artistic, or philosophical perspective. But where previously we had to rely on our intuition, we now have something more tangible to build upon.”

Although the findings were consistent across multiple songs and languages, the study’s musical selections were limited to the folk genre and to Western cultural contexts. Future work is needed to explore whether these effects generalize to other types of music—such as pop, jazz, or electronic—and to non-Western musical traditions.

“Ideally, we would have tested a much larger and more diverse set of music, in particular non-western music, and for each of them, included an expert familiar with that given music and culture,” Herff noted. “However, further increasing the stimulus set and number of recruited participants would have made this already logistically challenging endeavour unfeasible. But that is certainly something we have our eyes on for the future.”

It also remains unclear what specific musical features drive the effect. Is it melody, rhythm, tempo, or cultural associations that make a piece of music more likely to elicit social thought? Answering these questions would help refine music-based interventions in clinical and therapeutic settings.

“This study is part of a larger scale investigation into music-evoked mental imagery,” Herff said. “Currently we are investigation music-evoked mental imagery by looking both at very detailed musical features and how they shape mental imagery. For example, we just published this study: Micro-variations in timing and loudness affect music-evoked mental imagery. We are also looking closer at how listeners use music systematically to self-regulate. For example, in this recently published study, we explored how older adults engage with music when they feel lonely: Music as social surrogate? A qualitative analysis of older adults’ choices of music to alleviate loneliness.”

“At the same time, we are working closely together with the music community to understand the insights and intuitions on how to use music to shape listeners’ imagination that already exists in these experts. We hope that our research can contribute to clinical (e.g., cognitive behaviour therapies that use mental imagery techniques), recreational (e.g., roleplay), and artistic applications (e.g., new compositions).”

The study, “Solitary silence and social sounds: music can influence mental imagery, inducing thoughts of social interactions,” was authored by Steffen A. Herff, Gabriele Cecchetti, Petter Ericson, and Estefania Cano.

RELATED

Rapid neuroplasticity changes are associated with ketamine treatment response in patients with depression
Dementia

Scientists achieve “striking” memory improvements by suppressing brain protein

August 27, 2025

Scientists have identified a brain protein that appears to drive age-related memory decline. In a new study, suppressing this protein in old mice led to gains in cognitive performance, offering insight into potential therapies for brain aging.

Read moreDetails
A simple cognitive vaccine can make you more resistant to misinformation
Cognitive Science

A simple cognitive vaccine can make you more resistant to misinformation

August 26, 2025

A recent study tested whether people can be “inoculated” against misinformation by shifting how they think. The results suggest that boosting open-minded thinking through a simple message can reduce conspiracy beliefs and help individuals better separate truth from falsehood.

Read moreDetails
Pilates may help treat female sexual dysfunction, new study indicates
Cognitive Science

Letting loose with a swear word may actually make you stronger

August 25, 2025

Swearing out loud before a task boosted grip strength and increased motivational energy in a new psychology study. The findings suggest that taboo language can trigger emotional arousal and help direct focus toward immediate physical goals.

Read moreDetails
What we know about a person changes how our brain processes their face
Memory

Neuroscientists find evidence of an internal brain rhythm that orchestrates memory

August 24, 2025

A team of neuroscientists has observed that individual neurons in the human brain follow rhythmic timing patterns during memory tasks. The findings highlight how internal brain states influence when cells fire as people form and recall memories.

Read moreDetails
Machine learning algorithm identifies three unique autism subtypes in males
Cognitive Science

Evolution may have capped human brain size to balance energy costs and survival

August 24, 2025

Human brain growth slowed about 300,000 years ago, research in Brain & Cognition suggests. Energy demands and shifting climates may have capped brain size, pushing survival toward cultural innovations and cognitive offloading rather than ever-larger skulls.

Read moreDetails
Women feel unsafe when objectified—but may still self-sexualize if the man is attractive or wealthy
Cognitive Science

Children’s self-estimates of IQ become more accurate with age—but only to a point

August 23, 2025

Researchers found that children under 10 often misjudge their own mental abilities. By middle childhood, their self-assessments grow more accurate—though the link between self-perception and measured intelligence remains modest throughout adolescence.

Read moreDetails
Obesity before pregnancy linked to autism-like behavior in male offspring, study finds
Cognitive Science

Children fall for a surprisingly simple numerical illusion — and it grows stronger with age

August 19, 2025

A new study suggests that a simple visual trick—connecting pairs of dots—can make people see fewer objects than are actually present. Children as young as five show this illusion, and its strength increases into adulthood.

Read moreDetails
Astronaut sleep study reveals how spaceflight alters rest patterns
Cognitive Science

New research shows the weird ways zero gravity messes with your body awareness

August 18, 2025

Scientists studying people aboard parabolic flights found that weightlessness can scramble your sense of where your arms are — but only in some cases. The findings suggest the brain uses multiple systems to monitor body position, not just one.

Read moreDetails

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Autism’s “odd gait”: Autistic movement differences linked to brain development

Scientists achieve “striking” memory improvements by suppressing brain protein

Some neurocognitive deficits from COVID-19 may last for years, study suggests

Single dose of psilocybin provides lasting relief from depression and anxiety in cancer patients

Fascinating new psychology research shows how music shapes imagination

Surprising link found between aesthetic chills and political extremism

Insecurely attached individuals are less likely to go for a compromise in relationship conflicts

Conspiracy theories can significantly influence public support for war

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy