Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

High fluid intelligence boosts ability to accept corrected misinformation

by Eric W. Dolan
October 30, 2024
in Cognitive Science
(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Follow PsyPost on Google News

A recent study published in Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications sheds light on why some people are more likely to change their beliefs after being presented with corrections to misinformation. Researchers found that people with higher fluid intelligence, a form of general reasoning ability, were more likely to adjust their attitudes after receiving corrective information. Interestingly, an individual’s motivation to think deeply—what psychologists call “need for cognition”—had little effect on attitude change. This suggests that the ability to reason through new information is more influential than simply being motivated to engage with it.

The rise of misinformation, particularly online, has posed serious challenges for societies aiming to foster well-informed public discourse. In recent years, efforts to combat misinformation have often focused on fact-checking and correction strategies, but not all individuals respond to corrections in the same way. Understanding why some people adjust their beliefs while others do not could help improve debunking strategies.

The study’s authors wanted to explore whether personal characteristics like fluid intelligence (the ability to think and reason quickly and abstractly) or need for cognition (the desire to engage in effortful thinking) could predict who is more likely to update their attitudes in response to corrected information.

“The spread of misinformation is one of the key challenges for contemporary societies. Against this background, it seems important to understand what makes individuals fall for misinformation and what we can do about it,” said study author Fabian Hutmacher, a lecturer at the University of Würzburg.

The researchers conducted two online experiments, which were designed to simulate real-world misinformation correction scenarios, with a focus on minimizing pre-existing biases by choosing a neutral topic.

In the first experiment, the researchers recruited 401 participants through various online platforms. After applying exclusion criteria for inattention, incomplete responses, or self-reported low diligence, they finalized a sample of 355 participants aged 17 to 67, most of whom were university students.

Participants were divided into two groups: the experimental group, which received misinformation on a fictional topic — trust-based working hours — and a control group, which received a neutral, factual version of the same topic. Both groups read a news article about a company’s experiment with flexible working hours, but only the experimental group received misinformation suggesting that the project had negative consequences, like financial losses. Following this, the experimental group read a correction message that debunked the misinformation, presented as an independent fact-check that clarified the misinformation and reframed the project as generally positive.

The researchers measured participants’ attitudes toward trust-based working hours both before and after the correction message. Fluid intelligence was measured using a deductive reasoning test that involved analyzing logical conclusions, while need for cognition was assessed with a short survey where participants rated statements indicating their preference for complex or intellectually engaging tasks.

For the second experiment, the researchers aimed to expand and refine their findings from the first study by using a larger and more diverse sample. They recruited 795 participants from an online participant pool, Prolific, and applied similar exclusion criteria, ultimately resulting in a final sample of 725 participants.

The procedure in Study 2 was similar to Study 1, with participants again divided into experimental and control groups. The misinformation content, the neutral content, and the correction message were consistent with those used in the first study, ensuring comparability between the experiments. However, researchers made refinements to the measures of fluid intelligence and need for cognition. In this study, they used a longer, more reliable 16-item scale to measure need for cognition and standardized the fluid intelligence test items across participants to enhance the consistency and robustness of these measurements.

Across both experiments, the researchers found that individuals with higher fluid intelligence were more likely to adjust their attitudes after receiving a corrective message. The higher an individual’s fluid intelligence, the more effective they were at understanding and applying the corrective message to their initial beliefs.

“A common strategy for countering misinformation is correcting and fact-checking false information,” Hutmacher told PsyPost. “It has repeatedly been shown that a correction message diminishes but not always fully eliminates the effects of previously presented misinformation. We demonstrated that people with higher fluid intelligence, that is, people with a better general reasoning ability, are better at adjusting their attitude in response to the correction of misinformation.”

“Reversely put, this means that integrating a correction message with a previously encountered piece of misinformation can be challenging for individuals. From a practical perspective, this implies that it is important to communicate correction messages in a way that is accessible to a broad audience.”

Interestingly, need for cognition did not have a significant impact on the extent to which participants changed their attitudes in response to corrective information. Even among individuals with a high need for cognition—those who generally enjoy and actively engage in thinking—the presence of misinformation seemed to persist in their attitudes similarly to those with lower need for cognition.

“We did not only investigate whether fluid intelligence affects the way individuals react to the correction of misinformation,” Hutmacher said. “We also checked whether an individual’s need for cognition, that is, an individual’s willingness to engage in effortful thinking, plays a role in this context. However, we did not find any evidence that need for cognition is a significant contributing factor when it comes to the correction of misinformation.”

The sheds light on how certain cognitive trait may impact our ability to accept corrections and reduce the influence of misinformation. But, as with all research, there are some caveats. The misinformation used in the study was neutral, potentially making it easier for participants to accept the correction. In real-world contexts, however, misinformation often pertains to politically charged or personally significant issues. The authors suggest that future research should investigate how fluid intelligence and need for cognition influence responses to corrections on more controversial topics.

“When studying misinformation, one needs to present participants misinformation on a certain topic,” Hutmacher explained. “We presented our participants misinformation on the topic of trust-based working time. However, it would be important to extend our findings to more contested and more politicized topics such as climate change. Having a better understanding of what makes people fall for misinformation will hopefully help us to design more effective countermeasures.”

The study, “Fluid intelligence but not need for cognition is associated with attitude change in response to the correction of misinformation,” was authored by Fabian Hutmacher, Markus Appel, Benjamin Schätzlein, and Christoph Mengelkamp.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Poor sleep may shrink brain regions vulnerable to Alzheimer’s disease, study suggests
Memory

Neuroscientists discover biological mechanism that helps the brain ignore irrelevant information

June 14, 2025

New research suggests the brain uses a learning rule at inhibitory synapses to block out distractions during memory replay. This process enables the hippocampus to prioritize useful patterns over random noise, helping build more generalizable and reliable memories.

Read moreDetails
Brain boost from pecans? New study finds short-term cognitive benefits
Cognitive Science

Brain boost from pecans? New study finds short-term cognitive benefits

June 12, 2025

A new study published in Nutritional Neuroscience found that a pecan-enriched shake improved memory and attention in healthy young adults. Participants performed better on 8 of 23 cognitive tests after consuming pecans compared to a calorie-matched shake.

Read moreDetails
Democrats dislike Republicans more than Republicans dislike Democrats, studies find
Cognitive Science

New neuroscience study reveals sex-specific brain responses to threat

June 11, 2025

A new study shows that male and female mice engage distinct brain circuits when responding to threat, challenging the assumption that similar behavior reflects identical brain function. The findings highlight the need for sex-inclusive neuroscience research.

Read moreDetails
HIIT workouts outshine others in boosting memory and brain health, new study finds
Cognitive Science

Mega-study shows exercise boosts cognitive functioning across all ages and health conditions

June 11, 2025

From children to older adults, exercise enhances brainpower. A sweeping new analysis shows that physical activity improves general cognition, memory, and executive function in both healthy and clinical populations, reinforcing its value for mental sharpness at any age.

Read moreDetails
Democrats dislike Republicans more than Republicans dislike Democrats, studies find
Memory

Reduced memory specificity linked to earlier onset of psychiatric disorders in youth

June 11, 2025

New research suggests that difficulty recalling specific personal memories may be an early warning sign of mental illness in youth. A meta-analysis finds this memory trait predicts first-time psychiatric diagnoses, especially depression, during adolescence and early adulthood.

Read moreDetails
Psychopathy stands out as key trait behind uncommitted sexual behavior
Cognitive Science

Study identifies top-performing natural extracts for improving cognitive function

June 9, 2025

Researchers conducted a large-scale comparison of herbal supplements and found that certain natural extracts can improve memory, executive function, and cognitive flexibility in healthy adults—suggesting potential support for brain health through plant-based compounds.

Read moreDetails
Neuroimaging study suggests mindfulness meditation lowers sensory gating
Cognitive Science

Neuroimaging study suggests mindfulness meditation lowers sensory gating

June 7, 2025

A new study finds that mindfulness meditators are more likely to report feeling a touch — even when none occurs — and that this sensitivity is linked to altered brain rhythms.

Read moreDetails
Your brain’s insulation might become emergency energy during a marathon
Cognitive Science

Scientists map the hidden architecture of the brain’s default mode network

June 5, 2025

A new study reveals that the brain’s default mode network is made up of distinct anatomical types that support both internal thoughts and external processing. This structural diversity helps explain the network’s role in everything from memory to imagination.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Memes can serve as strong indicators of coming mass violence

9 psychology studies that reveal the powerful role of fathers in shaping lives

This self-talk exercise may help reduce emotional dysregulation in autistic children

Sleep problems top list of predictors for teen mental illness, AI-powered study finds

Scientists uncover surprisingly consistent pattern of scholarly curiosity throughout history

Single-dose psilocybin therapy shows promise for reducing alcohol consumption

Low-carb diets linked to reduced depression symptoms — but there’s a catch

Neuroscientists discover biological mechanism that helps the brain ignore irrelevant information

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy