Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology

Infants and toddlers infer close relationships from saliva sharing, study finds

by Patricia Y. Sanchez
June 16, 2022
in Social Psychology
Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Follow PsyPost on Google News

Some close relationships exhibit distinctive saliva-sharing interactions such as sharing food or kissing. New research published in Science found that infants and toddlers infer close and supportive relationships between those who engage in saliva sharing interactions.

People categorize social relationships by strength. Relationships that feature strong attachment, obligations, and mutual responsiveness (i.e., “thick” relationships) tend to be seen between close genetic relatives but can also exist outside the nuclear family. These relationships are also characterized by distinctive interactions such as those that transfer saliva (i.e., sharing food utensils).

“Here we apply experimental techniques from developmental science to test whether young children, toddlers, and infants do indeed infer that two individuals who share saliva are likely to be in a thick relationship,” wrote study author Ashley J. Thomas and colleagues.

In their first experiment, 113 children (aged 5 to 7 years old) were presented with interactions between cartoon people. Participants predicted that sharing utensils or licking the same food item would happen within a nuclear family, while sharing toys and food would happen both in friendships and in families. This suggests young children recognize this distinctive interaction as being indicative of a close relationship like that in a nuclear family.

In their next experiments, 27 toddlers (aged 16.5-18.5 months old) and 20 infants (aged 8.5-10 months old) saw a puppet alternately eat from the same orange slice as one actress (saliva sharing interaction) and play ball with another actress. The puppet then is seated between the two actresses and exhibits distress. Researchers recorded which actress the children look toward first and longer to measure which actress they expected to react to the puppet’s distress.

Results show both toddlers and infants looked at the actress who shared the orange slice first and longer than the other actress. In larger iteration of this study with a more racially diverse sample of 118 toddlers, the results were replicated. Children looked first and longer at the one who shared food with the puppet.

In their third series of experiments, researchers removed the element of food sharing and had the actress deliberately transfer her saliva to the puppet’s mouth using her finger. When interacting with a second puppet, the same actions were undertaken but with the puppet’s forehead. The actress then showed distress and researchers recorded their gaze toward each of the puppets.

Toddlers looked first and longer toward the puppet from the mouth interaction compared to the forehead interaction. Infants’ first looks were equal between the two puppets, but they looked longer at the mouth interaction puppet. When the actress was replaced with a new actor, infants and toddlers looked first and longer at the forehead interaction puppet.

A follow-up study of parents of infants and toddlers also found that parents expressed comfort with saliva-sharing interactions (i.e., sharing a utensil, drinking from the same cup) only in “thick” relationships.

“School-aged children’s judgments about saliva sharing are likely reinforced by explicit prohibitions (particularly during a pandemic), but similar intuitions appear to originate earlier and to generalize beyond the content of verbal rules motivated by hygiene,” the authors concluded. “We hypothesize that an early intuitive distinction between thick and thin relationships allows infants to rapidly learn the distinctive behaviors that occur in these relationships in their social environment.”

The authors cite some limitations to this work such as not including more measures to fully assess whether thick and thin relationships are thought of as distinct. Further, some interactions involving saliva transfer are aggressive (i.e., spitting on a person) and it was not assessed whether toddlers and infants can appreciate this difference.

The study, “Early concepts of intimacy: Young humans use saliva sharing to infer close relationships“, was authored by Ashley J. Thomas, Brandon Woo, Daniel Nettle, Elizabeth Spelke, and Rebecca Saxe.

TweetSendScanShareSendPin5ShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time
Racism and Discrimination

Hispanic adolescents experience later sleep timing and greater social jet lag than peers, study finds

July 3, 2025

Hispanic adolescents reported later sleep timing, greater social jet lag, shorter weekday sleep, higher caffeine intake, and more evening screen time than peers. Screen use and stress, but not caffeine, were linked to delayed sleep and sleep misalignment.

Read moreDetails
Racial and religious differences help explain why unmarried voters lean Democrat
Political Psychology

Student loan debt doesn’t deter civic engagement — it may actually drive it, new research suggests

July 3, 2025

Americans with student loan debt are more likely to vote and engage in political activities than those without debt, likely because they see government as responsible and capable of addressing their financial burden through policy change.

Read moreDetails
Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time
Infidelity

Not bothered by celebrity infidelity? This psychological trait might be why

July 3, 2025

The online shaming of unfaithful celebrities is a modern spectacle, but why do some join in while others don't? Researchers exploring this puzzle found a key predictor: a belief in a just world, which unexpectedly dampens outrage and blame.

Read moreDetails
Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time
Mental Health

New research suggests the conservative mental health advantage is a myth

July 3, 2025

Do conservatives really have better mental well-being than liberals? A new study suggests the answer depends entirely on how you ask. The well-known ideological gap disappears when "mental health" is replaced with the less-stigmatized phrase "overall mood."

Read moreDetails
New psychology study sheds light on mysterious “feelings of presence” during isolation
Political Psychology

People who think “everyone agrees with me” are more likely to support populism

July 1, 2025

People who wrongly believe that most others share their political views are more likely to support populist ideas, according to a new study. These false beliefs can erode trust in democratic institutions and fuel resentment toward political elites.

Read moreDetails
Researchers identify neural mechanism behind memory prioritization
Social Psychology

People who are more likely to die seem to care less about the future

June 30, 2025

Individuals with higher mortality risk—as judged by actuarial life insurance data—tend to care less about long-term consequences. They show more impulsivity and less future planning, consistent with evolutionary theories that link time horizon to environmental and internal health cues.

Read moreDetails
Scientists show how you’re unknowingly sealing yourself in an information bubble
Cognitive Science

Scientists show how you’re unknowingly sealing yourself in an information bubble

June 29, 2025

Scientists have found that belief polarization doesn’t always come from misinformation or social media bubbles. Instead, it often begins with a simple search. Our choice of words—and the algorithm’s response—can subtly seal us inside our own informational comfort zones.

Read moreDetails
Radical leaders inspire stronger devotion because they make followers feel significant, study finds
Political Psychology

Radical leaders inspire stronger devotion because they make followers feel significant, study finds

June 28, 2025

A new study finds that voters are more motivated by radical political leaders than moderates, because supporting bold causes makes them feel personally significant—driving greater activism, sacrifice, and long-term engagement across elections in the United States and Poland.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Hispanic adolescents experience later sleep timing and greater social jet lag than peers, study finds

Student loan debt doesn’t deter civic engagement — it may actually drive it, new research suggests

Understanding “neuronal ensembles” could revolutionize addiction treatment

Not bothered by celebrity infidelity? This psychological trait might be why

Genetic factors may influence how well exercise buffers against childhood trauma

Tips for parents in talking with your kids about your partner’s mental illness

Subjective cognitive struggles strongly linked to social recovery in depression

New research suggests the conservative mental health advantage is a myth

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy