Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

Listening to relaxing music can improve cognitive performance, study suggests

by Patricia Y. Sanchez
August 6, 2022
in Cognitive Science
Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Follow PsyPost on Google News

Relaxing background music has been shown to decrease both heart rate and respiration rate, which may positively affect cognitive performance. New research published in the Journal of Cognitive Enhancement found that listening to three genres of relaxing music (jazz, piano, and lo-fi) may improve cognitive performance.

Research shows that listening to different types of music can improve sustained attention, alertness, and attentional focus. However, other studies show that background music may disrupt cognitive performance (i.e., text comprehension, verbal memory).

For the current study, study author Ulrich Kirk and colleagues were interested in comparing whether different types of relaxing background music could affect cognitive processing and physiological activity. “The study recruited four groups of participants where each group was exposed to one specific genre of music compared to a no-music control group. In a between-group design, the study exposed three separate groups to jazz music, piano music, and lo-fi music respectively. The fourth group was a no-music control group.”

The researchers sampled 108 adult participants with no heart or stress conditions for this study. Each participant was randomly assigned to one of the four experimental groups. The study took place over three days where participants were measured for mind wandering (sustained attention), acute attention, and heart rate variability (HRV). Importantly, participants were measured for acute attention while listening to music and measured for sustained attention after listening to music.

On the first day, participants completed baseline measures of sustained attention and HRV. On the second day, participants were taken to a room, given headphones, and listened to music corresponding to their experimental condition while also being monitored for HRV. They were also measured for acute attention during the last 5 mins of music listening and for sustained attention when the session was over.

On the third day, participants repeated the procedure from day 2 and listened to the same music again. The only difference is that some participants listened to a 15-minute clip on day 2 and then a 45-minute clip on day 3 and other participants listened in the opposite order. Three weeks later, participants came back to complete another 15-minute music session and attention task. Participants were instructed to listen to their assigned piece of music at least 10 times over the three weeks to increase familiarity with the music.

Results show that those who listened to music (regardless of length) had higher performance compared to the no-music control group. Further, those who listened to music (all three genres) showed an increase in performance over the study period for both 15- and 45-minute music sessions.

Similarly, those who listened to music (regardless of length) showed higher HRV compared to the no-music control group. There was an increase in HRV over the study period for those who listened to music, but this increase was observed in the no-music control group as well. These differences were observed for both the 15- and 45-minute conditions.

Results from the follow-up test three weeks later show that those who listened to music had faster reaction times compared to the no-music control group. Results also show that those in the music groups displayed an improvement in reaction time at the follow-up compared to those in the no-music control group who showed no differences. Lastly, those in the no-music control group had the lowest HRV at follow-up compared to the other three music groups.

The researchers cite some limitations to this work, such as not including an active-control group such as rock music. Future research showing that music that is not relaxing can impair performance can boost confidence in these results. Another limitation is not measuring how participants felt about the music they were listening to. Perhaps being fond of music in general can boost performance.

The study, “Effects of Three Genres of Focus Music on Heart Rate Variability and Sustained Attention“, was authored by Ulrich Kirk, Christelle Ngnoumen, Alicia Clausel, and Clare Kennedy Purvis.

RELATED

Lonely individuals show greater mood instability, especially with positive emotions, study finds
Cognitive Science

Game-based training can boost executive function and math skills in children

August 16, 2025

Children who played the Cucca Curiosa game showed improvements in working memory, cognitive flexibility, and math. The findings suggest that digital interventions designed to strengthen executive functioning may also indirectly support academic skills like arithmetic and problem-solving.

Read moreDetails
Lonely individuals show greater mood instability, especially with positive emotions, study finds
Cognitive Science

Researchers identify a key pathway linking socioeconomic status to children’s reading skills

August 16, 2025

New research published in Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience suggests that children’s reading skills are shaped more by their language environment than by structural brain differences. The study found that vocabulary and phonological awareness explain the influence of parental education on reading performance.

Read moreDetails
Positivity resonance predicts lasting love, according to new psychology research
Cognitive Science

Common infections during pregnancy unlikely to impact children’s intelligence, large study finds

August 15, 2025

Using data from over a quarter-million siblings, researchers found little evidence that common infections during pregnancy influence children’s intelligence or school performance, suggesting that most antibiotic use in pregnancy is unlikely to harm long-term cognitive development.

Read moreDetails
Positivity resonance predicts lasting love, according to new psychology research
Memory

Scientists uncover brain’s “reset button” for splitting memories into distinct events

August 15, 2025

A small brainstem region known as the locus coeruleus appears to help the brain segment experiences into distinct memories. New research links this neural activity to pupil responses and changes in hippocampal patterns during meaningful event transitions.

Read moreDetails
Neuroscientists identify a reversible biological mechanism behind drug-induced cognitive deficits
Cognitive Science

Dopamine’s role in learning may be broader than previously thought

August 11, 2025

New research reveals dopamine helps the brain juggle fast, flexible problem-solving with gradual habit formation. By boosting working memory use and enhancing trial-and-error learning, dopamine influences both how quickly we learn and how costly mental effort feels.

Read moreDetails
Exercise can reduce feelings of hopelessness among patients in suicide crisis, pilot study finds
Cognitive Science

Physically active individuals tend to have slightly better cognitive abilities on average

August 10, 2025

A new meta-analysis suggests that while physical activity generally has a small positive impact on cognition, outdoor moderate-to-vigorous exercise paired with cognitively challenging activities delivers the biggest gains across age groups, from preschoolers to older adults.

Read moreDetails
People with narcissistic tendencies report more ostracism and are more often excluded
Anxiety

Sleep may amplify negative memory bias in anxious youth

August 10, 2025

Researchers have found that children and young adolescents with higher anxiety tend to generalize negative memories more after sleep, raising questions about how nighttime memory processes could reinforce anxiety-related thought patterns during a sensitive developmental stage.

Read moreDetails
Moderate aerobic exercise enhances the brain’s “eighth sense”
Cognitive Science

Bright children from low-income homes lose cognitive edge in early secondary school

August 8, 2025

A new UK study finds that bright 5-year-olds from low-income families match their affluent peers academically through primary school, but between ages 11 and 14, they face steep declines in motivation, behavior, mental health, and exam performance.

Read moreDetails

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Esketamine nasal spray shows rapid antidepressant effects as standalone treatment

Game-based training can boost executive function and math skills in children

Gabapentin use for back pain linked to higher risk of dementia, study finds

Researchers identify a key pathway linking socioeconomic status to children’s reading skills

These fascinating new studies show ADHD extends into unexpected areas

A woman’s craving for clay got so intense it mimicked signs of addiction

Lonely individuals show greater mood instability, especially with positive emotions, study finds

Study hints cannabis use may influence sleep test results, raising concerns about misdiagnosis

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy