Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology Political Psychology

Partisan news habits strikingly more common in the United States than Japan or Hong Kong

by Eric W. Dolan
December 4, 2024
in Political Psychology
(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

A new study published in Communication Research has uncovered striking differences in how people from different countries interact with political news. In the United States, individuals overwhelmingly choose news that aligns with their partisan views, reinforcing political divides. However, this pattern is far less pronounced in Hong Kong and nearly absent in Japan, raising questions about the cultural and political factors that influence selective news consumption.

The rise of polarized media and the ease of customizing news consumption have raised concerns about the widening divide in political attitudes, particularly in the United States. Partisan selective exposure, where individuals prefer news that aligns with their views, has been linked to political polarization as it reinforces existing biases.

However, much of the research in this area has been conducted in the United States, where a unique two-party system, partisan media environment, and high levels of political polarization prevail. Researchers aimed to determine whether this behavior is a universal human tendency or a phenomenon shaped by specific cultural and political conditions.

“Research on selective exposure, often considered a contributing factor to political polarization, has been predominantly led by scholars in the United States,” explained study author Tetsuro Kobayashi, a professor of political science and economics at Waseda University.

“While partisan selective exposure is robustly observed in the U.S., I suspected that this phenomenon might not be as pronounced in Asia. Despite this, discussions in Japan and other Asian contexts often took the findings from U.S.-based studies for granted. This prompted me to conduct comparative research to uncover cross-national differences rather than simply assuming the universality of U.S.-centric findings.”

The research consisted of two studies.

In Study 1, Kobayashi and his colleagues aimed to measure partisan selective exposure—the tendency to choose news that aligns with one’s political preferences—across the United States, Japan, and Hong Kong. They used a controlled design involving mock news websites to observe participants’ browsing behavior.

These websites displayed eight headlines, with four political and four non-political stories. The political headlines focused on each region’s political leader: Donald Trump in the United States, Shinzo Abe in Japan, and Carrie Lam in Hong Kong. Among the political headlines, two were positive and two were negative about the leader, allowing participants the option to select either pro-attitudinal or counter-attitudinal content.

Participants were given 90 seconds to explore the site, during which their headline clicks and time spent reading articles were unobtrusively recorded. This time constraint ensured participants could not read all articles, compelling them to make choices that reflected their preferences.

The sample included over 600 participants from each region, recruited through online platforms to ensure diverse and comparable demographic representation. Each participant’s approval or disapproval of their region’s leader was measured beforehand, enabling researchers to categorize clicks and reading times as either pro- or counter-attitudinal.

The results of Study 1 revealed stark differences in media selection behavior across the three regions. Participants in the United States showed a strong preference for pro-attitudinal headlines, clicking on and spending more time with content that aligned with their political views. This behavior was consistent with the concept of partisan selective exposure.

In contrast, Japanese participants showed almost no preference, selecting pro- and counter-attitudinal headlines in nearly equal measure. Participants from Hong Kong exhibited an intermediate level of selective exposure, with a modest preference for pro-attitudinal content but not to the same extent as their U.S. counterparts.

“I anticipated that partisan selective exposure in Japan would be weaker than in the United States, but I did not expect it to be so minimal as to be nearly non-existent,” Kobayashi told PsyPost. “Former Prime Minister Shinzo Abe was one of the rare political leaders in Japan who elicited polarized evaluations, leading me to expect some degree of partisan selective exposure. However, this did not materialize in the data.”

These findings highlighted cross-national differences in the strength of partisan selective exposure, with the United States standing out as a unique case. However, while Study 1 confirmed that selective exposure was more pronounced in the United States, it did not explore the factors driving these differences. This limitation led the researchers to design Study 2 to better understand the underlying mechanisms behind the observed patterns.

Study 2 built on the findings of the first study and sought to investigate potential explanations for the cross-national differences in partisan selective exposure. Kobayashi and his colleagues hypothesized that affective polarization might contribute to these differences. They also examined the potential roles of perceived media credibility and cultural factors, such as the tendency to reconcile opposing viewpoints.

This time, participants’ emotional polarization was assessed through a survey measuring their feelings toward political in-groups and out-groups. For example, U.S. participants rated their feelings toward supporters of the Democratic and Republican parties, while Japanese participants rated their feelings toward those who approved or disapproved of Prime Minister Shinzo Abe.

Media trust was measured through questions about the fairness, accuracy, and trustworthiness of the media in each region. Cultural attitudes toward accepting contradictions were assessed using a standardized questionnaire on dialectical thinking, which evaluates individuals’ willingness to reconcile seemingly opposing perspectives.

The findings from Study 2 revealed that affective polarization partly explained the observed cross-national differences in selective exposure. U.S. participants, who exhibited the strongest selective exposure, also displayed the highest levels of affective polarization.

Japanese participants, on the other hand, showed the lowest levels of polarization and the weakest selective exposure. Hong Kong participants, with moderate levels of polarization, fell between the two. This pattern suggested that strong emotional biases against political opponents might drive individuals to avoid counter-attitudinal content.

“The proliferation of the internet and social media does not deterministically amplify partisan selective exposure,” Kobayashi explained. “Instead, the intensification of selective exposure occurs primarily in politically polarized contexts. In the United States, polarization stems from various causes and has been a longstanding trend predating the advent of the internet. It is reasonable to consider a bidirectional causal relationship between partisan selective exposure and political polarization.”

Interestingly, the researchers found that perceived media credibility and cultural attitudes toward contradictions did not significantly explain the cross-national differences in selective exposure. While previous studies have linked low trust in media to biased information consumption, this relationship was not evident in this study. Similarly, the cultural tendency of East Asians to reconcile contradictions did not appear to suppress selective exposure in Japan or Hong Kong.

“A significant portion of the cross-country differences remains unexplained,” Kobayashi told PsyPost. “The inability to fully identify these unexplained differences is one of the key limitations of this study.”

The findings also underscore the need for cross-national research to develop a more nuanced understanding of political communication and polarization.

“Many theories and models in political communication research are grounded in the U.S. political system,” Kobayashi said. “However, as this study demonstrates, phenomena robustly observed in the U.S. may not manifest in the same way in Asia. My long-term goal is to develop theories and models of political communication that are firmly rooted in the realities of Asian political systems.”

The study, “Is Partisan Selective Exposure an American Peculiarity? A Comparative Study of News Browsing Behaviors in the United States, Japan, and Hong Kong,” was authored by Tetsuro Kobayashi, Zhifan Zhang, and Ling Liu.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Poor sleep may shrink brain regions vulnerable to Alzheimer’s disease, study suggests
Political Psychology

Christian nationalists tend to imagine God as benevolent, angry over sins, and engaged

June 14, 2025

Christians who believe God is benevolent, engaged, and angered by sin are more likely to support religious nationalism, according to a new study. This worldview was also associated with conspiracy mentality and xenophobic attitudes.

Read moreDetails
Fear predicts authoritarian attitudes across cultures, with conservatives most affected
Authoritarianism

Fear predicts authoritarian attitudes across cultures, with conservatives most affected

June 13, 2025

A sweeping study of over 84,000 people across 59 countries found that individuals who feel threatened by crime, poverty, or instability are more likely to support authoritarian governance—especially in Western nations and among politically right-leaning individuals.

Read moreDetails
New research links certain types of narcissism to anti-immigrant attitudes
Narcissism

New research links certain types of narcissism to anti-immigrant attitudes

June 13, 2025

New research published in Behavioral Sciences shows that certain narcissistic traits are linked to anti-immigrant attitudes through competitive worldviews and ideological beliefs, highlighting the role of personality in shaping how people view immigrants and social hierarchies.

Read moreDetails
Uncertainty about immigration intensifies nationalist sentiments
Political Psychology

From well-being to white replacement: What psychology research says about immigration

June 11, 2025

Immigration sparks fierce debates—but what does the research say? These seven studies uncover surprising findings about mental health, crime, social media, nationalism, and political violence.

Read moreDetails
Democrats dislike Republicans more than Republicans dislike Democrats, studies find
Political Psychology

TikTok “edits” can reshape how voters see politicians, study suggests

June 11, 2025

A new study finds that TikTok-style “edits” portraying politicians as attractive or powerful can sway public perceptions, boosting attractiveness and favorability—especially for Donald Trump. These videos may signal a shift in how political influence is crafted and consumed.

Read moreDetails
Democrats dislike Republicans more than Republicans dislike Democrats, studies find
Political Psychology

Democrats dislike Republicans more than Republicans dislike Democrats, studies find

June 11, 2025

Across multiple studies, researchers found Democrats express more negative social judgments toward Republicans. The driving factor appears to be a moral belief that Republicans harm disadvantaged people, such as racial minorities, fueling stronger partisan dislike on the political left.

Read moreDetails
Political ambivalence has a surprising relationship with support for violence
Authoritarianism

Perceived social breakdown fuels desire for authoritarian leaders, new psychology study shows

June 10, 2025

New research provides causal evidence that perceived societal breakdown—known as anomie—can increase support for authoritarianism by eroding feelings of control and increasing uncertainty.

Read moreDetails
New research shows 2020 U.S. vote counts were extraordinarily accurate, contradicting fraud claims
Political Psychology

New research shows 2020 U.S. vote counts were extraordinarily accurate, contradicting fraud claims

June 10, 2025

A new study provides one of the most comprehensive checks of 2020 U.S. vote accuracy to date, showing that audited ballots closely matched original counts and that claims of widespread miscounts or fraud are not supported by the evidence.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Conspiracy believers tend to overrate their cognitive abilities and think most others agree with them

Memes can serve as strong indicators of coming mass violence

9 psychology studies that reveal the powerful role of fathers in shaping lives

This self-talk exercise may help reduce emotional dysregulation in autistic children

Sleep problems top list of predictors for teen mental illness, AI-powered study finds

Scientists uncover surprisingly consistent pattern of scholarly curiosity throughout history

Single-dose psilocybin therapy shows promise for reducing alcohol consumption

Low-carb diets linked to reduced depression symptoms — but there’s a catch

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy