Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

Perception of ‘illusory’ depth altered in astronauts during spaceflight

by David Hayward
October 29, 2015
in Cognitive Science
Photo credit: NASA

Photo credit: NASA

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

Astronauts’ brains begin to process visual information differently as they become acclimated to a weightless environment, a new study published in PLoS One reports.

Optical illusions made up of line drawings that can be seen in two different ways, for example a chair that can be seen either facing towards or away from you, are known as “reversible perspective figures.” Studying how people see these figures sheds light on how the human brain takes two-dimensional images from the eyes and puts them together to see in three dimensions.

When people look at these illusions, what they see tends to switch back and forth between the two possible visual interpretations, but one of these views is typically dominant and is seen about 70 percent of the time. This is thought to occur because of the way the brain interprets cues about visual depth. Because the ground is usually closer to us than the sky or ceiling, the brain is more prepared to see lower parts of the drawing as closer, making it easier to see the picture in one way than the other.

A new study, led by Gilles Clément of the Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, studied how astronauts saw these illusions to find out how prolonged weightlessness affects human depth perception. Six astronauts living on the International Space Station were tested on their vision of four reversible perspective figure optical illusions, viewing the figures on a screen and reporting each time their vision switched between the two possible interpretations. The astronauts were tested three times before leaving Earth, four times while in space for a period of up to six months, and another three times after returning.

Reversible perspective figures: a man’s face or old woman begging (left); a Spartan soldier head and helmet or a golfer swinging (right).
Reversible perspective figures: a man’s face or old woman begging (left); a Spartan soldier head and helmet or a golfer swinging (right).

Before going into space, the astronauts’ perception of the figures was the same as other people’s, with the dominant interpretation being seen about 70 percent of the time. Once they were in space, the astronauts’ perceptions began to change gradually over time. Instead of seeing one interpretation most of the time, they began to see both possibilities roughly equally. Their vision remained the same immediately after they returned to Earth, but after nine months they had returned to seeing one interpretation of the figure dominantly over the other.

According to the study authors, these results reveal that “depth is not so much the direct result of certain specifiable stimulus cues as it is a mental construction.” The brain pieces together information on visual depth based partly on experience with the environment. In an environment with gravity, this experience tells us that objects in the lower part of the field of vision are probably closer than those in the upper part. For someone who is weightless, the upper field of vision is just as likely to be close as the lower part, giving the brain fewer clues to depth.

In addition to advancing our understanding of how three dimensional vision is processed by the brain, these insights may help future astronauts combat “space fog,” the sense of confusion and disorientation reported after spending time in a weightless environment.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

The fading affect bias impacts most memories — but election-related memories are surprisingly resilient
Memory

Scientists shed light on how forgiveness does and doesn’t reshape memories

June 20, 2025

A new study suggests that forgiving someone does not make us forget what they did—but it does change how we feel about it. People who forgave recalled past wrongs with just as much detail, but with less emotional pain.

Read moreDetails
Tree-covered neighborhoods linked to lower ADHD risk in children
Cognitive Science

Scientists demonstrate superior cognitive benefits of outdoor vs indoor physical activity

June 18, 2025

A new study suggests that where kids exercise matters: children who played basketball outside showed sharper thinking and faster reaction times than when playing indoors, hinting at a powerful brain-boosting synergy between physical activity and nature.

Read moreDetails
Scientists uncover biological pathway that could revolutionize anxiety treatment
Cognitive Science

Different parts of the same neuron learn in different ways, study finds

June 16, 2025

Researchers have discovered that apical and basal dendrites of the same neuron use different strategies to learn, suggesting neurons adapt more flexibly than previously thought. The findings help explain how the brain fine-tunes its wiring during learning.

Read moreDetails
Poor sleep may shrink brain regions vulnerable to Alzheimer’s disease, study suggests
Memory

Neuroscientists discover biological mechanism that helps the brain ignore irrelevant information

June 14, 2025

New research suggests the brain uses a learning rule at inhibitory synapses to block out distractions during memory replay. This process enables the hippocampus to prioritize useful patterns over random noise, helping build more generalizable and reliable memories.

Read moreDetails
Brain boost from pecans? New study finds short-term cognitive benefits
Cognitive Science

Brain boost from pecans? New study finds short-term cognitive benefits

June 12, 2025

A new study published in Nutritional Neuroscience found that a pecan-enriched shake improved memory and attention in healthy young adults. Participants performed better on 8 of 23 cognitive tests after consuming pecans compared to a calorie-matched shake.

Read moreDetails
Democrats dislike Republicans more than Republicans dislike Democrats, studies find
Cognitive Science

New neuroscience study reveals sex-specific brain responses to threat

June 11, 2025

A new study shows that male and female mice engage distinct brain circuits when responding to threat, challenging the assumption that similar behavior reflects identical brain function. The findings highlight the need for sex-inclusive neuroscience research.

Read moreDetails
HIIT workouts outshine others in boosting memory and brain health, new study finds
Cognitive Science

Mega-study shows exercise boosts cognitive functioning across all ages and health conditions

June 11, 2025

From children to older adults, exercise enhances brainpower. A sweeping new analysis shows that physical activity improves general cognition, memory, and executive function in both healthy and clinical populations, reinforcing its value for mental sharpness at any age.

Read moreDetails
Democrats dislike Republicans more than Republicans dislike Democrats, studies find
Memory

Reduced memory specificity linked to earlier onset of psychiatric disorders in youth

June 11, 2025

New research suggests that difficulty recalling specific personal memories may be an early warning sign of mental illness in youth. A meta-analysis finds this memory trait predicts first-time psychiatric diagnoses, especially depression, during adolescence and early adulthood.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Psychopathic individuals recognize unfairness but are less likely to punish it

The neuroscience of why we cry happy tears

Scientists shed light on how forgiveness does and doesn’t reshape memories

Epistemic mistrust and dogmatism predict preference for authoritarian-looking leaders

Rare post-orgasm illness causes days-long flu-like symptoms, but research into its cause remains limited

Gut bacteria may play a causal role in obsessive-compulsive disorder, study suggests

Regular cannabis use linked to changes in brain activity regulating movement

Do AI tools undermine our sense of creativity? New study says yes

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy