Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Mental Health Anxiety

Reflexive fear responses tend to linger in people with anxiety disorders, study suggests

by Vladimir Hedrih
February 18, 2023
in Anxiety
Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

People with and without anxiety disorders learn to fear a threat equally quickly, according to new research. However, people with anxiety disorders tend to have a harder time learning to stop being afraid on a physiological level (when the threat is gone) compared to healthy individuals. These trends can be detected by monitoring fear-potentiated startle responses.

The study was published in the International Journal of Psychophysiology.

Learning to recognize threats is a capacity that is of extreme importance for survival. However, problems in these processes, such as situations when an individual easily learns to associate certain stimuli with danger but fails to extinguish this association once the stimuli in question are not longer associated with danger, have long been linked to the development and maintenance of pathological anxiety.

Some theorists have linked the fact that anxiety symptoms start developing in late childhood and early adolescence with variations in the maturation of neural circuitry supporting threat learning. This is often explored experimentally by using the so-called differential threat conditioning study plans.

These research studies pair one neutral stimulus with another that is clearly unpleasant or painful (so-called unconditional stimulus) and present another neutral stimulus just by itself. They then compare the fear reaction to the neutral stimulus that was paired with the unpleasant/painful one to the fear reaction (or its absence) to the neutral stimulus that was presented alone.

Scientists most often use two different indicators of fear – skin conductance response and fear-potentiated startle response.

Skin conductance response assesses fear through measuring the conductivity of the skin. When a person is afraid (or more precisely – aroused, either positively or negatively), the sympathetic part of the autonomic nervous system activates sweating, skin becomes more moist, thus increasing conductance (water is a conductor).

The fear startle response is a reflexive physiological reaction to a stimulus one perceives as a threat. It is most often measured by monitoring eye blinks – a person will tend to blink when something he/she perceives as dangerous approaches.

Study author Philip Newsome and his colleagues wanted to explore whether anxiety effects on differential threat conditioning and extinction (learning to stop reacting with fear to a neutral stimulus that one previously associated with danger) can be detected by monitoring a specific type of fear-potentiated startle response.

They reanalyzed a dataset from a 2020 study containing responses of 306 persons. Of the sample, 133 were diagnosed with anxiety or a similar disorder, while the remaining patients were diagnosis-free. The threat conditioning was done by presenting pictures of two women (two neutral stimuli) to participants.

After the first picture was presented, another picture of the same woman was presented soon after, but the woman in this new picture showed fear. This was accompanied by a 95-decibel female scream delivered via headphones. The picture of the other woman was presented just by itself. Each picture was presented 4 times for 8 seconds.

Fear response was measured through fear-potentiated startle. 4-10 psi of compressed air were blown on the forehead of the study participant 5-6 seconds after showing a picture of a women and an electromyogram recorded whether the participant blinked.

The researchers additionally recorded skin conductance responses using two electrodes attached to middle and ring fingers of the participant’s non-dominant hand. Participants also gave ratings of their subjective feeling of fear. They collected data on the treatment outcome of participants diagnosed with anxiety (or a similar disorder). Finally, researchers analyzed data from magnetic resonance images of participants’ brains, but these were available only for 205 participants.

Results showed that both the group of patients with an anxiety diagnosis and the group without a diagnosis successfully learned fear conditioning responses. In other words, they quickly learned to react with fear to the picture that was followed by the loud scream.

This finding was obtained both by monitoring the fear-potentiated startle response and skin conductance. Participants diagnosed with anxiety on average reported being more afraid than diagnosis-free participants. Older participants reported being less afraid.

When learning to extinguish the threat reaction was considered, results of skin conductance response monitoring pointed to similar rates of diminishing response to the picture that used to be accompanied by a loud scream. As trials during which the picture was not followed by the scream proceeded, fear reaction measured through skin conductance gradually decreased.

However, when fear-potentiated startle responses were examined, results showed that participants with the an anxiety disorder diagnosis extinguished the fear response much slower than participants from the diagnosis-free group. Even at the end of the procedure, the fear-potentiated startle response was higher for the picture that used to be associated with the loud scream. A similar finding was obtained when self-reported fear was compared between the two groups.

“Together, fear-potentiated startle responses, but not skin conductance responses, reveal retainment of threat contingencies in pathological anxiety. These findings suggest that fear-potentiated startle responses and skin conductance responses might capture distinct processes related to conditioned threat responding,” the study authors concluded.

The study gives an important contribution to scientific knowledge on the physiology of fear responses. However, it should be noted that the lack of effects of anxiety on the skin conductance response might have to do with the fear extinction phase being too short. Additionally, it is possible that methods of measuring fear were themselves somewhat unpleasant. This might have affected the validity of fear monitoring data.

The study, “Fear-potentiated startle reveals diminished threat extinction in pathological anxiety”, was authored by Philip Newsome, Sonia G. Ruiz, Andrea L. Gold, Daniel S. Pine, and Rany Abend.

TweetSendScanShareSendPin1ShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

New research shows the psychological toll of the 2024 presidential election
Anxiety

New research shows the psychological toll of the 2024 presidential election

July 13, 2025

Among young adults, stress from election news was linked to higher risks of depression and anxiety, while pre-election anticipatory stress was linked to depression only. Stress about the election outcome was not associated with either condition.

Read moreDetails
Liberals and conservatives live differently — but people think the divide is even bigger than it is
Anxiety

Highly irritable teens are more likely to bully others, but anxiety mitigates this tendency

July 12, 2025

Social anxiety weakens the link between irritability and bullying in adolescents—irritable teens are less likely to bully others if they also have social anxiety. This moderating effect was not seen with generalized anxiety or other forms of aggression.

Read moreDetails
Adults with ADHD face significantly shorter life expectancy, study finds
Anxiety

Study finds “Anxious Mondays” linked to long-term stress and heart health risks in older adults

July 10, 2025

Researchers have discovered that anxiety felt on Mondays is associated with higher long-term cortisol levels in older adults, suggesting the start of the week may contribute to biological stress in ways that extend far beyond the office.

Read moreDetails
Ketamine repairs reward circuitry to reverse stress-induced anhedonia
Anxiety

Virtual reality meditation eases caregiver anxiety during pediatric hospital stays, with stronger benefits for Spanish speakers

July 9, 2025

Researchers at Stanford found that a six-minute guided meditation delivered through a VR headset lowered anxiety levels in parents of hospitalized children. The intervention outperformed standard relaxation methods, with Spanish-speaking caregivers experiencing the greatest benefit.

Read moreDetails
Heightened anxiety linked to an impaired ability to simulate alternative versions of past events
Anxiety

New research reveals emotional control deficits in generalized anxiety disorder

July 7, 2025

A new study suggests that individuals with generalized anxiety disorder have difficulty focusing on relevant emotional information and shifting attention between emotional tasks, highlighting impairments in affective control that may contribute to chronic anxiety symptoms.

Read moreDetails
Liver health may influence mental health via inflammation and glutamate levels
Anxiety

Liver health may influence mental health via inflammation and glutamate levels

June 28, 2025

A new study suggests that diets high in fat and fructose can damage the liver and trigger anxiety-like behaviors in mice. The research also found that corilagin, a natural compound, reversed many of these harmful effects.

Read moreDetails
How people end romantic relationships: New study pinpoints three common break up strategies
Anxiety

Spider fear inflates size perception, highlighting the role of emotion in threat assessment

June 26, 2025

A new study finds that people afraid of spiders perceive them as larger than they really are, while arachnology experts judge their size accurately. The findings shed light on how emotion and knowledge shape perception of threatening creatures.

Read moreDetails
Anxious minds don’t always fixate on danger, according to new study
Anxiety

Anxious minds don’t always fixate on danger, according to new study

June 25, 2025

A new study challenges long‑held ideas about anxiety and attention. It finds that when motivated, anxious people can shift focus from threats just as quickly as others — suggesting that “dwelling” on threats may be more about circumstance than an inherent deficit.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

The triglyceride-glucose index: Can it predict depression risk in the elderly?

People with ADHD exhibit altered brain activity before making high-stakes choices

Narcissistic individuals are more prone to maladaptive daydreaming

Effort or fate? Sexual mindsets predict whether bedroom bliss spills over into broader wellbeing

People who use AI may pay a social price, according to new psychology research

Researchers identify a shared brain pattern behind feeling surprised

New research shows the psychological toll of the 2024 presidential election

Methylphenidate: ADHD drug curbs impulsivity in men only, linked to brain wiring differences

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy