Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive COVID-19

Study finds that 30% of people subscribed to a fact-checking newsletter have recently shared misinformation

by Beth Ellwood
August 27, 2021
in COVID-19, Social Media
Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Follow PsyPost on Google News

New findings published in PLOS One suggest that even people who are highly concerned about fake news are susceptible to sharing it. Among a sample of individuals who were subscribed to a COVID-19-related fact-checking newsletter, about 30% had shared debunked information at least five times in the past 3 months.

The widespread circulation of misinformation or “fake news” related to COVID-19 continues to cause concern among health officials around the world. Misinformation about the virus can cause citizens to question the efficacy of public health measures aimed at curbing the spread of the disease, such as mask-wearing and COVID-19 vaccines.

Researchers Lauren L. Saling and her team wanted to explore the psychological factors that make people susceptible to sharing misinformation while focusing on a sample of individuals who should be less willing to share such content. Specifically, the researchers explored the prevalence of misinformation sharing and the motivations for sharing misinformation among a sample of people subscribed to a fact-checking newsletter.

“All of the authors of this research are members of RMIT FactLab, which is the research arm of a fact checking body (RMIT ABC Fact Check — a joint initiative between the Australian Broadcasting Corporation and RMIT University),” explained Saling.

“The broad research aims of RMIT FactLab are as follows: i. to establish which people or groups are particularly susceptible to fake news and misinformation and inclined to share fake news and misinformation, ii. to determine the platforms on which fake news and misinformation are shared, iii. to determine the kinds of information that are more likely to be shared, and iv. to determine the optimal way to present debunked information to increase its efficacy in combatting misinformation and fake news.”

Saling and her colleagues recruited 1,397 Australians who were subscribed to a newsletter created by a fact-checking organization in Australia. The newsletter, called CoronaCheck, publishes weekly content that debunks misinformation related to COVID-19. The newsletter subscribers completed an online questionnaire where they answered questions about their engagement with the newsletter, engagement with fake news, attitudes toward COVID-19, belief in science, and conspiracy thinking.

“In this study, we deliberately included only subscribers to CoronaCheck, a weekly fact checking newsletter produced by RMIT ABC Fact Check presenting fact checked information predominantly around COVID-19,” Saling explained. “Given that such individuals are clearly concerned about fake news and misinformation and actively seek debunked information, it would seem unlikely that they would be inclined to share misinformation.”

Compared to general samples, the CoronaCheck subscribers had a higher belief in science and a lower conspiracy mentality. Overall, the respondents were highly concerned about fake news, perceived expert information about COVID-19 to be trustworthy, and felt they were good at discriminating between fake news and genuine news. Around 72% said they read the fact-checking newsletter every time they received it.

Interestingly, despite being highly concerned with fake news and having subscribed to a fact-checking newsletter, many participants were guilty of sharing misinformation. About 30% indicated that they had shared debunked information at least five times in the past 3 months. Similarly, 31% said they had shared information that they later found out to be false, and 24% said they had shared possible misinformation (information they did not know to be true or untrue).

“The sharing of misinformation is done even by people who are aware of fake news and misinformation and concerned about information veracity,” Saling told PsyPost. “Sharing of misinformation and fake news is very problematic as even if it is shared in good faith, sharing it exposes others to the information which can engender false beliefs. Once beliefs are established, they are very hard to shift.”

The most common reasons for sharing misinformation were to get a second opinion on whether or not a claim was true (38%) and because it seemed interesting (37%). Another 12% said they shared misinformation for entertainment purposes.

The study’s authors emphasize that sharing misinformation, even if one’s intentions are positive, still contributes to the circulation of false information. Even if a person shares a fake news article to point out inaccuracies or to discredit it, these efforts are lost once it gets re-shared. The false information then has the potential to reach many new readers who may then develop inaccurate beliefs.

“It is puzzling that sharing of possible misinformation persists in a cohort who are both attuned to and concerned about misinformation and who actively seek the debunking of misinformation. Sharing misinformation, even when it is not done to deceive, increases the chances that it will promote faulty beliefs in others,” Saling and her team say.

Although fact-checking newsletters likely increase people’s vigilance to fake news, it appears they do not necessarily keep people from spreading misinformation. The authors say that, instead, a more explicit intervention may be needed.

“The next studies in our research program involve using experience sampling methodology to ask people to report at multiple time points what information they are sharing, motivations for sharing and platforms on which information is shared, and using eye tracking to investigate how people interact with information in order to determine the optimal way to present debunked information to increase its efficacy,” Saling said.

The study, “No one is immune to misinformation: An investigation of misinformation sharing by subscribers to a fact-checking newsletter”, was authored by Lauren L. Saling, Devi Mallal, Falk Scholer, Russell Skelton, and Damiano Spina.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Daughters who feel more attractive report stronger, more protective bonds with their fathers
Social Media

Moral outrage spreads petitions online—but doesn’t always inspire people to sign them

July 15, 2025

Posts expressing moral outrage were more likely to go viral but did not lead to more petition signatures, while posts using agentic, group identity, or prosocial language attracted more signatures despite receiving less online engagement.

Read moreDetails
The most popular dementia videos on TikTok tend to have the lowest quality, study find
Addiction

People with short-video addiction show altered brain responses during decision-making

July 8, 2025

People who frequently use short-video apps like TikTok may show reduced loss sensitivity and impulsive decision-making, according to a new neuroimaging study that links addictive use patterns to changes in brain activity during risky choices.

Read moreDetails
New psychology study sheds light on mysterious “feelings of presence” during isolation
COVID-19

Frequent dreams and nightmares surged worldwide during the COVID-19 pandemic

July 1, 2025

An international study of over 15,000 adults across 16 countries found that dream recall and nightmares became more common during the pandemic, with sleep duration, age, and gender all playing a role in how often people experienced them.

Read moreDetails
Scientists show how you’re unknowingly sealing yourself in an information bubble
Cognitive Science

Scientists show how you’re unknowingly sealing yourself in an information bubble

June 29, 2025

Scientists have found that belief polarization doesn’t always come from misinformation or social media bubbles. Instead, it often begins with a simple search. Our choice of words—and the algorithm’s response—can subtly seal us inside our own informational comfort zones.

Read moreDetails
TikTok tics study sheds light on recovery trends and ongoing mental health challenges
Body Image and Body Dysmorphia

TikTok and similar platforms linked to body dissatisfaction and eating disorder symptoms

June 27, 2025

Frequent use of platforms like TikTok and YouTube Shorts is linked to disordered eating symptoms among teens, according to new research. The study found that body comparisons and dissatisfaction may help explain this troubling association—especially among girls.

Read moreDetails
Loneliness skews partner perceptions, harming relationships and reinforcing isolation
Mental Health

Maximization style and social media addiction linked to relationship obsessive compulsive disorder

June 24, 2025

Researchers have identified connections between obsessive thoughts about relationships, emotional closeness, and habits like social media addiction and striving for perfection. The findings highlight risk factors that can deepen doubt and tension in romantic connections, especially when conflict is present.

Read moreDetails
It’s not digital illiteracy: Here’s why older adults are drawn to dubious news
Social Media

Believing “news will find me” is linked to sharing fake news, study finds

June 22, 2025

People who rely on social media to “stumble upon” news are more prone to spreading misinformation, according to a new longitudinal study.

Read moreDetails
Some dark personality traits may help buffer against depression, new psychology research suggests
COVID-19

COVID-19 coverage linked to rise in anti-Asian sentiment, especially among Trump supporters

June 17, 2025

Americans became less favorable toward Asians as COVID-19 spread and news coverage intensified, according to a new study. The drop was strongest among Trump supporters, highlighting how political rhetoric and fear shaped public opinion during the pandemic.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Women favor men with attractive faces when making social bargaining decisions

Caffeine increases brain complexity during sleep, study shows

Psychedelic retreats show promise in easing depression, PTSD, and reintegration struggles among veterans

Neurons in an autism model fail to distinguish social from non-social touch

Medicinal cannabis may actually worsen sleep, a new study finds

Scientists identify the brain’s built-in brake for binge drinking

Trump’s speeches stump AI: Study reveals ChatGPT’s struggle with metaphors

Childhood maltreatment linked to emotion regulation difficulties and teen mental health problems

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy