Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

The surprising age when cognitive skills actually peak (and how to keep them strong)

by Eric W. Dolan
March 12, 2025
in Cognitive Science
Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

A new study published in Science Advances challenges the widely held belief that cognitive skills begin to decline starting as early as age 30. Instead, researchers found that literacy and numeracy skills generally improve until at least age 40, after which they either stabilize or decline slightly. However, this decline is not inevitable. People who engage in frequent skill-related activities at work and in everyday life tend to maintain or even improve their cognitive abilities beyond their forties.

The researchers conducted this study to provide a more accurate picture of how cognitive skills change with age. Most previous research relied on cross-sectional data, which compares people of different ages at a single point in time. This approach can be misleading because it does not account for generational differences in education and skill development.

For example, an older person might score lower on a literacy test than a younger person, but this could reflect differences in their educational background rather than a true decline in ability over time. The researchers wanted to separate the effects of aging from these cohort differences by tracking the same individuals over time.

“Cognitive skills have a strong impact on an individual’s labor market earnings and on economic growth of countries. The aging of advanced societies around the world raises concerns about the pattern of skills with individual age. But the existing research had obvious limitations because it did not follow the skills of individuals but instead simply compared individuals of different ages,” explained study author Eric A. Hanushek, a senior fellow at the Hoover Institution of Stanford University.

For their new study, the researchers used data from the German longitudinal component of the Programme for the International Assessment of Adult Competencies (PIAAC-L). This project followed a large, representative sample of German adults who were tested on literacy and numeracy skills twice, with a gap of 3.5 years between assessments. This allowed the researchers to measure real changes in individual skills rather than relying on indirect comparisons between different age groups.

Skill usage was measured through a detailed questionnaire included in the initial PIAAC assessment. Participants reported how frequently they engaged in various reading and math-related activities both at work and in daily life. For reading, they indicated how often they read instructions, emails, articles, books, and reference materials. For numeracy, they reported activities such as calculating costs, using fractions or percentages, and interpreting graphs. Responses were recorded on a five-point scale, ranging from “never” to “every day.”

The research team used statistical methods to account for a common issue in repeated testing: measurement error. When people take tests, their scores are not perfectly accurate. There is always some degree of random fluctuation. This fluctuation can create a misleading impression of skill change over time, a phenomenon known as “reversion to the mean.”

Imagine someone scores very low on a test simply due to bad luck or random error. On a retest, their score is likely to be closer to their true skill level, making it seem like their skills have improved, even if they haven’t. The researchers employed statistical adjustments to minimize this bias and get a more accurate estimate of genuine skill changes with age.

After applying these adjustments, the researchers discovered a pattern quite different from what previous studies had suggested. On average, literacy and numeracy skills actually increased substantially into people’s forties. After this peak, literacy skills showed only a slight decline, while numeracy skills decreased more noticeably. However, these averages hid an important variation. When the researchers looked at skill changes in relation to how frequently people used their literacy and numeracy skills in their daily lives and at work, a striking difference emerged.

For individuals who reported using their skills more often than average, there was no sign of skill decline within the age range studied (up to age 65). In fact, their literacy and numeracy skills continued to improve into their fifties and then leveled off. In contrast, skill decline was primarily observed in individuals who reported below-average skill usage. This suggests that actively using cognitive skills throughout adulthood may be a key factor in maintaining or even improving them.

“Individual behavior dramatically affects the age pattern of cognitive skills,” Hanushek told PsyPost. “Those who use literacy or numeracy skills at home or work can lessen if not eliminate neurological patterns of skill decline.”

“The role of skill usage holds across a wide range of circumstances — across occupations, education levels, and life style differences such as quality of diet and exercise options. Within demographic and behavioral groups, those who use skills in normal activities tend to delay any aging effects on cognitive skills.”

The researchers also examined how these patterns differed across different groups of people. They found that individuals in white-collar jobs and those with higher levels of education, groups known to use their skills more frequently, showed increasing skill levels even beyond their forties, as long as they reported above-average skill usage. However, if people in these groups had below-average skill usage, this positive trend was not observed.

Another interesting finding was that women, on average, tended to experience larger skill losses at older ages, particularly in numeracy. This gender difference was not completely explained by differences in skill usage, suggesting other factors might be at play.

As with any study, there are some limitations to consider. The data only included adults up to age 65, so it cannot tell us about skill changes in older age groups. Also, the study was conducted in Germany, and it is uncertain whether these findings would be the same in other countries with different cultural and educational systems. Future research could investigate skill changes in older populations and in different countries to see how generalizable these results are.

In addition, the study focused only on literacy and numeracy. Future research could explore how other cognitive skills change with age and whether skill usage influences them in similar ways. While the study used robust statistical methods and longitudinal data, it remains observational, meaning it cannot establish cause and effect.

Despite these limitations, this research provides important insights into cognitive aging. It suggests that cognitive decline is not a uniform or inevitable process. Actively engaging our minds and regularly using our literacy and numeracy skills throughout adulthood may play a significant role in preserving and even enhancing these abilities as we age. This offers a more encouraging perspective on aging and highlights the potential for lifelong learning and cognitive engagement to support healthy cognitive function throughout life.

“This work is part of a long term research program into the determinants of cognitive skill differences and the economic and social impacts of any differences,” Hanushek explained. “We see more and more that skill differences have strong impacts on both individuals and nations.”

The study, “Age and cognitive skills: Use it or lose it,” was authored by Eric A. Hanushek, Lavinia Kinne, Frauke Witthöft, and Ludger Woessmann.

RELATED

Pilates may help treat female sexual dysfunction, new study indicates
Cognitive Science

Letting loose with a swear word may actually make you stronger

August 25, 2025

Swearing out loud before a task boosted grip strength and increased motivational energy in a new psychology study. The findings suggest that taboo language can trigger emotional arousal and help direct focus toward immediate physical goals.

Read moreDetails
What we know about a person changes how our brain processes their face
Memory

Neuroscientists find evidence of an internal brain rhythm that orchestrates memory

August 24, 2025

A team of neuroscientists has observed that individual neurons in the human brain follow rhythmic timing patterns during memory tasks. The findings highlight how internal brain states influence when cells fire as people form and recall memories.

Read moreDetails
Machine learning algorithm identifies three unique autism subtypes in males
Cognitive Science

Evolution may have capped human brain size to balance energy costs and survival

August 24, 2025

Human brain growth slowed about 300,000 years ago, research in Brain & Cognition suggests. Energy demands and shifting climates may have capped brain size, pushing survival toward cultural innovations and cognitive offloading rather than ever-larger skulls.

Read moreDetails
Women feel unsafe when objectified—but may still self-sexualize if the man is attractive or wealthy
Cognitive Science

Children’s self-estimates of IQ become more accurate with age—but only to a point

August 23, 2025

Researchers found that children under 10 often misjudge their own mental abilities. By middle childhood, their self-assessments grow more accurate—though the link between self-perception and measured intelligence remains modest throughout adolescence.

Read moreDetails
Obesity before pregnancy linked to autism-like behavior in male offspring, study finds
Cognitive Science

Children fall for a surprisingly simple numerical illusion — and it grows stronger with age

August 19, 2025

A new study suggests that a simple visual trick—connecting pairs of dots—can make people see fewer objects than are actually present. Children as young as five show this illusion, and its strength increases into adulthood.

Read moreDetails
Astronaut sleep study reveals how spaceflight alters rest patterns
Cognitive Science

New research shows the weird ways zero gravity messes with your body awareness

August 18, 2025

Scientists studying people aboard parabolic flights found that weightlessness can scramble your sense of where your arms are — but only in some cases. The findings suggest the brain uses multiple systems to monitor body position, not just one.

Read moreDetails
Lonely individuals show greater mood instability, especially with positive emotions, study finds
Cognitive Science

Game-based training can boost executive function and math skills in children

August 16, 2025

Children who played the Cucca Curiosa game showed improvements in working memory, cognitive flexibility, and math. The findings suggest that digital interventions designed to strengthen executive functioning may also indirectly support academic skills like arithmetic and problem-solving.

Read moreDetails
Lonely individuals show greater mood instability, especially with positive emotions, study finds
Cognitive Science

Researchers identify a key pathway linking socioeconomic status to children’s reading skills

August 16, 2025

New research published in Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience suggests that children’s reading skills are shaped more by their language environment than by structural brain differences. The study found that vocabulary and phonological awareness explain the influence of parental education on reading performance.

Read moreDetails

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Letting loose with a swear word may actually make you stronger

Pilates may help treat female sexual dysfunction, new study indicates

Is ChatGPT making us stupid?

Children raised in poverty are less likely to believe in a just world

Religious attendance linked to greater support for youth tackle football, study finds

Virtual workout partners may not be real but they still feel real enough to boost your exercise

New research identifies multiple personal, social, and biological risk factors for PTSD

Psilocybin and MDMA may reset fear-related brain-immune signaling, scientists find

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy