Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology

The unexpected consequence of “watching eyes”: A study on survey misreporting in China

by Eric W. Dolan
April 5, 2024
in Social Psychology
(Photo credit: OpenAI's DALL·E)

(Photo credit: OpenAI's DALL·E)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Don't miss out! Follow PsyPost on Bluesky!

A recent study published in Discover Psychology has shed light on the peculiar effects of psychological interventions on survey honesty. Researchers in China set out to examine the factors related to misreporting in online health surveys and to assess the efficacy of using images of “watching eyes” to deter such behavior. Contrary to expectations, the study found that these images not only failed to decrease misreporting but actually increased the likelihood of false responses.

The internet has revolutionized the way researchers collect data, with platforms like Amazon Mechanical Turk facilitating easy access to millions of potential respondents. However, the integrity of survey responses remains a concern, especially when compensation is involved, tempting some to misreport for quicker completion.

Misreporting can severely undermine the validity of research findings, leading to misguided public health policies and resource misallocation. In China, the use of Sojump for participant recruitment has grown, yet little was known about the extent or factors contributing to survey misreporting within this context.

“When we started conducting another study in China using online surveys, I became curious about two issues. (1) How concerned should we be about deliberate misreporting by survey respondents? And (2) How effective would images of watching eyes be at discouraging deliberate misreporting? To the best of my knowledge, there was very limited research on survey misreporting in China, and no published studies that tested watching eyes to specifically discourage survey misreporting,” explained study author Stephen W. Pan, an assistant professor at The University of Texas at San Antonio.

The study included 1,655 Chinese adults who were recruited via Sojump. These participants were randomly divided into four groups. Specifically, participants were either asked to report their blood type directly or told they could skip the rest of the survey by claiming to have type AB blood, introducing a temptation to misreport.

Half of the groups were exposed to a “watching eyes” graphic before answering, intended to subconsciously encourage honesty. This setup aimed to both identify demographic predictors of misreporting and test the surveillance cue’s efficacy in a controlled online environment.

“We detected very limited evidence of deliberate survey misreporting when respondents were presented with an incentive to do so,” Pan told PsyPost. “Granted, the incentive to misreport was quite weak (an opportunity to end the survey early), but this finding is reassuring to us and perhaps other researchers who collect data through online survey organizations in China.”

A significant distinction emerged between participants exposed to the watching eyes imagery and those who were not, especially among those given an incentive to misreport their blood type. Contrary to the researchers’ hypothesis, the group exposed to the watching eyes and presented with the opportunity to misreport for a quicker survey completion were more likely to provide false information.

“We found that the effect of watching eyes images completely backfired,” Pan explained. “It seems that the watching eyes were so off-putting, that respondents exposed the watching eyes were actually MORE likely to misreport answers, simply so they could end the survey early. Hence, it appears that images of watching eyes can decrease positive engagement and cooperation, depending on the incentive structures.”

Across the board, no significant sociodemographic predictors of misreporting were identified, indicating that the propensity to misreport under the influence of watching eyes did not vary meaningfully with factors like age, gender, or education level.

The study is not without its limitations. The incentive for misreporting, tied to survey completion rather than a more tangible reward, may not have been strong enough to capture the full extent of dishonest behavior. Additionally, the sample lacked diversity in educational attainment, potentially overlooking the effects of socioeconomic status on survey honesty.

“We conducted a follow-up misreporting that included stronger incentives,” Pan noted. “Hoping to have that out in the near future.”

The study, “Who misreports on internet health surveys, and do images of watching eyes discourage misreporting? An online experiment from China,” was authored by Tianyu Guo, Chuqing Cao, Jing Wang, Xinwen Hu, Ying Chen, and Stephen W. Pan.

RELATED

Psychopathic men and less selective women report more sex via Tinder
Dating

Psychopathic men and less selective women report more sex via Tinder

August 26, 2025

New research finds that men high in psychopathy and sexual desire, and women who are less picky with matches, report more sexual encounters via Tinder. The findings suggest dating apps favor fast, opportunistic mating strategies shaped by personality.

Read moreDetails
Early social rejection may foster dark personality traits through loneliness, study suggests
Early Life Adversity and Childhood Maltreatment

Children raised in poverty are less likely to believe in a just world

August 25, 2025

A new longitudinal study of Chinese high school students suggests that growing up in poverty weakens belief in a just world, while unpredictable environments show less consistent effects. Perceptions of discrimination appear to help explain this link.

Read moreDetails
Acetaminophen use during pregnancy linked to higher risk of autism, ADHD in children
Psychology of Religion

Religious attendance linked to greater support for youth tackle football, study finds

August 25, 2025

Frequent churchgoers are more likely to say tackle football is appropriate for kids, according to new research. The study highlights how religious beliefs may influence support for the sport.

Read moreDetails
Study finds Trump and Harris used distinct rhetoric in 2024—but shared more similarities than expected
Political Psychology

Study finds Trump and Harris used distinct rhetoric in 2024—but shared more similarities than expected

August 24, 2025

Donald Trump and Kamala Harris framed the 2024 presidential debate in starkly different terms, according to a new study—but their language also showed surprising overlap in tone, emotional content, and specificity.

Read moreDetails
Women feel unsafe when objectified—but may still self-sexualize if the man is attractive or wealthy
Attractiveness

Women feel unsafe when objectified—but may still self-sexualize if the man is attractive or wealthy

August 23, 2025

New research from China suggests that women feel unsafe when confronted with a sexually objectifying gaze—but still choose to self-sexualize if the man is attractive or high status. The findings highlight a psychological tradeoff between risk and potential reward.

Read moreDetails
The most popular dementia videos on TikTok tend to have the lowest quality, study find
Social Media

Most TikTok videos about birth control are unreliable, study finds

August 23, 2025

TikTok is flooded with misleading content about contraception, according to a new study. Most viral videos are not made by medical experts and often promote “natural” methods while casting doubt on hormonal options and professional medical advice.

Read moreDetails
Smash or pass? AI could soon predict your date’s interest via physiological cues
Artificial Intelligence

Researchers fed 7.9 million speeches into AI—and what they found upends our understanding of language

August 23, 2025

A massive linguistic study challenges the belief that language change is driven by young people alone. Researchers found that older adults often adopt new word meanings within a few years—and sometimes even lead the change themselves.

Read moreDetails
Americans broadly agree on what’s “woke,” but partisan cues still shape perceptions
Political Psychology

Americans broadly agree on what’s “woke,” but partisan cues still shape perceptions

August 22, 2025

Do Americans agree on what “woke” means? A new study suggests yes—up to a point. The term tends to signal different things depending on political identity, especially around race, gender, and alignment with the Democratic Party.

Read moreDetails

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

New psychology research identifies factors that predict sexual dream intensity

Can facial fillers and Botox-like injections improve your dating prospects or how others see you?

A simple cognitive vaccine can make you more resistant to misinformation

Psychopathic men and less selective women report more sex via Tinder

Letting loose with a swear word may actually make you stronger

Pilates may help treat female sexual dysfunction, new study indicates

Is ChatGPT making us stupid?

Children raised in poverty are less likely to believe in a just world

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy