Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Evolutionary Psychology

Why do women cheat? New study reveals complex motivations behind female infidelity

by Eric W. Dolan
July 28, 2024
in Evolutionary Psychology, Infidelity
(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Don't miss out! Follow PsyPost on Bluesky!

A recent study published in Evolution and Human Behavior offers new insights into the complex motivations behind female infidelity. The findings reveal that women who cheat are typically more physically attracted to their affair partners but view their primary partners as better co-parents. This suggests that women may cheat to obtain “good genes” for their offspring while relying on their primary partners for parenting support. Additionally, the study highlights several other motivations for infidelity, including relationship dissatisfaction, the desire for variety, and revenge.

Understanding why women commit infidelity is an ongoing debate in evolutionary psychology. Traditionally, infidelity in men has been explained through the desire to increase reproductive success by mating with multiple partners. However, this explanation does not translate directly to women, who do not gain the same reproductive benefits from having multiple mates. A woman’s reproductive output is limited by the number of pregnancies she can have, not by the number of mates she has, unlike men who can potentially father many more offspring with multiple partners.

Consequently, having multiple mates does not significantly increase a woman’s reproductive success in the same way it does for men. This discrepancy has led scientists to explore other potential evolutionary reasons for female infidelity, resulting in two main hypotheses: the mate-switching hypothesis and the dual-mating strategy.

The mate-switching hypothesis suggests that women might engage in infidelity to find a better long-term partner, essentially laying the groundwork to replace their current mate with someone of higher mate value. On the other hand, the dual-mating strategy posits that women might seek to obtain the best possible genetic material from an affair partner while retaining the parental investment from their primary partner. This strategy would mean prioritizing physical attractiveness and genetic benefits in affair partners while valuing their primary partners for their parental qualities.

“It might sound funny, but the evolutionary drivers of female infidelity in humans is an area of vigorous debate in my part of academia. In a way, there are too many good explanations for it!” said study author Macken Murphy, a PhD student at the University of Melbourne and host of the “Species” podcast.

The researchers used Prolific Academic to survey a sample of 254 heterosexual English-speaking men and women who had engaged in infidelity. The survey collected data from participants across 19 countries, making the sample more representative of different socioecological contexts compared to previous studies often limited to specific populations like American undergraduates.

The survey measured three main components of attraction — physical (appearance and sexual desirability), personal (how much they liked their partners’ personal qualities and enjoyed their company), and parental (perceived parenting abilities and suitability for raising children together) — towards both the affair partner and the primary partner using modified versions of the Interpersonal Attraction Scale and the Mate Value Scale. These scales are well-established in the literature and were chosen for their reliability and validity.

In addition to quantitative measures, the survey included qualitative questions to gather more nuanced data about participants’ motivations for infidelity. Participants were asked to describe, in their own words, what motivated them to have an affair. This qualitative component allowed the researchers to capture a broader range of motivations and to identify recurrent themes and patterns in the reasons people cheat.

Women in the study generally rated their affair partners as more physically attractive but less suitable as co-parents compared to their primary partners. This pattern aligns with the dual-mating strategy, where the primary goal of an affair is to combine “good genes” from an affair partner with the parenting support of the primary partner.

Contrary to the mate-switching hypothesis, the study found no evidence that women preferred their affair partners overall or saw them as better long-term mates. Women did not rate their affair partners higher in terms of mate value or personality compared to their primary partners. This indicates that women are not typically seeking to replace their primary partners with their affair partners. Instead, they seem to maintain a clear distinction between the roles of their primary and affair partners, valuing different qualities in each.

Interestingly, the study also found that men exhibited similar patterns to women in their infidelity behavior. This suggests that men, like women, may also engage in infidelity to seek genetic benefits from physically attractive affair partners while valuing their primary partners for parental investment.

“Finding that men were strategically dualistic as well — prioritizing physical attraction in affair partners and parental ability in primary partners — was a surprise,” Murphy told PsyPost. “However, it’s consistent with some past research, some of which suggests men are even more dualistic than women in their affairs.”

The qualitative data provided further insights into the diverse motivations behind infidelity. Some participants’ responses aligned with the dual-mating strategy. For example, several women mentioned being physically attracted to their affair partners and wanting to experience being with someone more attractive than their primary partner.

Although the findings did not support the mate-switching hypothesis overall, some participants indicated that they had affairs in search of a better long-term partner. A few women mentioned falling in love with their affair partners or wanting to be with them long-term, which reflects the mate-switching strategy. This indicates that while mate-switching may not be the dominant motive, it is still relevant for a subset of individuals.

Both men and women cited a desire for variety and novelty as a reason for their affairs. This motivation aligns with the idea that individuals seek new and exciting experiences outside their primary relationships. For instance, one woman mentioned being bored in her relationship, and a man described wanting to “sow his wild oats” while still young. These responses suggest that the drive for new sexual or romantic experiences can be a significant factor in infidelity.

A significant number of participants, particularly women, mentioned relationship dissatisfaction as a key motivator for their affairs. This included feelings of neglect, lack of emotional support, and general unhappiness with their primary partners. Women were notably more likely than men to cite their partner’s lack of investment and support as reasons for cheating.

Some participants reported infidelity as a form of revenge against their primary partners, particularly in response to their partner’s infidelity. Women were more likely than men to cite revenge as a motivation, reflecting a desire to retaliate and restore a sense of justice or balance. For example, one woman mentioned discovering her partner’s emails seeking other women, which prompted her to cheat as a form of payback.

A few participants indicated that their affairs were driven by a desire to validate their attractiveness and desirability. For example, one woman wanted to know that other men still found her desirable and wanted to gain more sexual experience. This reflects a motive to acquire information about one’s own value and attractiveness in the mating market.

Although less commonly cited, the multiple investors hypothesis was supported by a few responses. This hypothesis suggests that women might engage in infidelity to secure additional resources and support from multiple partners, thereby enhancing their offspring’s survival and well-being. This strategy is particularly relevant in environments where the primary partner’s investment is insufficient, prompting women to seek supplementary help from other men. For example, one woman mentioned getting closer to another man who was always available to help her and her child, suggesting she sought additional investment and support outside her primary relationship.

Finally, some participants mentioned unique or situational factors that influenced their decision to cheat. These included stress, long-distance relationships, and the influence of drugs and alcohol. These responses highlight that infidelity can be influenced by a range of external and situational factors, beyond the primary evolutionary strategies.

“Infidelity is a tactic that serves a variety of evolutionarily coherent strategies, including obtaining additional resources, switching to a new primary mate, and, especially in our study, acquiring genetic benefits for offspring,” Murphy said. “However, while humans evolved to cheat, that doesn’t mean we should, and most people don’t.”

But as with all research, there are some limitations. For example, the study relied on self-reported data, which can be influenced by social desirability bias, meaning participants might not fully disclose socially unacceptable motivations for their infidelity.

In addition, Murphy highlighted three important caveats to consider: “1) most women don’t cheat, and many women who do aren’t pursuing a dual mating strategy, and so the vast majority of women are not enacting a dual mating strategy, 2) this study isn’t about conscious motivations, directly, but the evolved psychology underlying them, and 3) the phrase ‘good genes’ comes from evolutionary biology, so it can sound fairly crazy when applied to humans. ‘Good,’ here, is meant in a fitness sense, not in a moral sense — nobody has better genes than anybody else.”

It is also important to note that providing an evolutionary explanation for a behavior, such as infidelity, is not the same as justifying or excusing it. “Just because cheating is natural doesn’t make it alright,” Murphy said.

The study, “Why women cheat: testing evolutionary hypotheses for female infidelity in a multinational sample,” was authored by Macken Murphy, Caroline A. Phillips, and Khandis R. Blake.

RELATED

People high in psychopathy and low in cognitive ability are the most politically active online, study finds
Evolutionary Psychology

Scientists uncover dozens of genetic traits that depend on which parent you inherit them from

August 20, 2025

A groundbreaking study in Nature suggests that some genes affect us differently depending on whether they’re inherited from our mother or father. Researchers identified dozens of parent-specific genetic effects related to traits like height, metabolism, and disease risk.

Read moreDetails
Traditional beliefs can shift the link between beauty and women’s sexual openness, new research suggests
Attractiveness

Traditional beliefs can shift the link between beauty and women’s sexual openness, new research suggests

August 14, 2025

Physically attractive women tend to report greater interest in casual sex, but a new study finds that traditional moral values—especially those emphasizing social order and purity—can override this tendency, particularly among women with rural or conservative backgrounds.

Read moreDetails
A simple verbal hint can alter a person’s visual perception, especially if they are highly suggestible
Attractiveness

Scientists map the visual patterns people use when evaluating others’ bodies

August 1, 2025

Researchers have used eye-tracking to show that our gaze is not random when we evaluate bodies. A study in Behavioral Sciences found that attention is focused on the chest and torso, and this focus shifts when judging for attractiveness, health, or youth.

Read moreDetails
Is evolutionary psychology a scientific revolution or an evolving paradigm?
Evolutionary Psychology

The psychology of belief explains America’s ongoing war with Darwin

July 29, 2025

One hundred years post-Scopes trial, why does the evolution debate persist in the US? It may have less to do with faith and more to do with our brains. Research shows fundamentalism can suppress analytical thinking, making facts alone unpersuasive.

Read moreDetails
Fascinating new research reveals how sexual desire shapes long-term partner preferences
Attractiveness

Fascinating new research reveals how sexual desire shapes long-term partner preferences

July 29, 2025

Psychologists have discovered that sexual desire plays a key role in long-term partner preferences. When desire increases, women’s interest in physical attractiveness rises to match men's, reducing long-standing sex differences in what people look for in committed relationships.

Read moreDetails
Systematic review finds causal association between childhood maltreatment and mental health problems
Evolutionary Psychology

New psychology research challenges influential theory linking childhood poverty to risk-taking

July 28, 2025

Growing up poor might slightly influence how adults respond to threats, but a large replication study found much weaker effects than past research suggested. The results call into question earlier claims about poverty, risk-taking, and decision-making.

Read moreDetails
Scientists link common “forever chemical” to male-specific developmental abnormalities
Dating

Attractive long-term mates have a weird unexpected effect on women’s creativity

July 24, 2025

Women’s creativity got a boost from thinking about committed relationships—but only to a point. A new study suggests that sexual arousal linked to highly desirable long-term partners may short-circuit creative thinking, highlighting a strange cost of romantic attraction.

Read moreDetails
An ancient survival instinct may explain why we crave junk food during uncertain times
Evolutionary Psychology

An ancient survival instinct may explain why we crave junk food during uncertain times

July 23, 2025

New research isolates a key mechanism underlying food choice in uncertain environments. An experiment found that an individual's preconceived belief in future food scarcity, when combined with cues of economic hardship, significantly increases visual fixation on high-calorie food items.

Read moreDetails

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

A new frontier in autism research: predicting risk in babies as young as two months

Cerebellar-prefrontal brain connectivity may shape negative symptoms in psychosis

Children’s self-estimates of IQ become more accurate with age—but only to a point

Women feel unsafe when objectified—but may still self-sexualize if the man is attractive or wealthy

Most TikTok videos about birth control are unreliable, study finds

Researchers fed 7.9 million speeches into AI—and what they found upends our understanding of language

Americans broadly agree on what’s “woke,” but partisan cues still shape perceptions

Scientists rewired people’s romantic “type” using a made-up trait—here’s what happened next

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy