Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive COVID-19

Study finds that 30% of people subscribed to a fact-checking newsletter have recently shared misinformation

by Beth Ellwood
August 27, 2021
in COVID-19, Social Media
Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay informed on the latest psychology and neuroscience research—follow PsyPost on LinkedIn for daily updates and insights.

New findings published in PLOS One suggest that even people who are highly concerned about fake news are susceptible to sharing it. Among a sample of individuals who were subscribed to a COVID-19-related fact-checking newsletter, about 30% had shared debunked information at least five times in the past 3 months.

The widespread circulation of misinformation or “fake news” related to COVID-19 continues to cause concern among health officials around the world. Misinformation about the virus can cause citizens to question the efficacy of public health measures aimed at curbing the spread of the disease, such as mask-wearing and COVID-19 vaccines.

Researchers Lauren L. Saling and her team wanted to explore the psychological factors that make people susceptible to sharing misinformation while focusing on a sample of individuals who should be less willing to share such content. Specifically, the researchers explored the prevalence of misinformation sharing and the motivations for sharing misinformation among a sample of people subscribed to a fact-checking newsletter.

“All of the authors of this research are members of RMIT FactLab, which is the research arm of a fact checking body (RMIT ABC Fact Check — a joint initiative between the Australian Broadcasting Corporation and RMIT University),” explained Saling.

“The broad research aims of RMIT FactLab are as follows: i. to establish which people or groups are particularly susceptible to fake news and misinformation and inclined to share fake news and misinformation, ii. to determine the platforms on which fake news and misinformation are shared, iii. to determine the kinds of information that are more likely to be shared, and iv. to determine the optimal way to present debunked information to increase its efficacy in combatting misinformation and fake news.”

Saling and her colleagues recruited 1,397 Australians who were subscribed to a newsletter created by a fact-checking organization in Australia. The newsletter, called CoronaCheck, publishes weekly content that debunks misinformation related to COVID-19. The newsletter subscribers completed an online questionnaire where they answered questions about their engagement with the newsletter, engagement with fake news, attitudes toward COVID-19, belief in science, and conspiracy thinking.

“In this study, we deliberately included only subscribers to CoronaCheck, a weekly fact checking newsletter produced by RMIT ABC Fact Check presenting fact checked information predominantly around COVID-19,” Saling explained. “Given that such individuals are clearly concerned about fake news and misinformation and actively seek debunked information, it would seem unlikely that they would be inclined to share misinformation.”

Compared to general samples, the CoronaCheck subscribers had a higher belief in science and a lower conspiracy mentality. Overall, the respondents were highly concerned about fake news, perceived expert information about COVID-19 to be trustworthy, and felt they were good at discriminating between fake news and genuine news. Around 72% said they read the fact-checking newsletter every time they received it.

Interestingly, despite being highly concerned with fake news and having subscribed to a fact-checking newsletter, many participants were guilty of sharing misinformation. About 30% indicated that they had shared debunked information at least five times in the past 3 months. Similarly, 31% said they had shared information that they later found out to be false, and 24% said they had shared possible misinformation (information they did not know to be true or untrue).

“The sharing of misinformation is done even by people who are aware of fake news and misinformation and concerned about information veracity,” Saling told PsyPost. “Sharing of misinformation and fake news is very problematic as even if it is shared in good faith, sharing it exposes others to the information which can engender false beliefs. Once beliefs are established, they are very hard to shift.”

The most common reasons for sharing misinformation were to get a second opinion on whether or not a claim was true (38%) and because it seemed interesting (37%). Another 12% said they shared misinformation for entertainment purposes.

The study’s authors emphasize that sharing misinformation, even if one’s intentions are positive, still contributes to the circulation of false information. Even if a person shares a fake news article to point out inaccuracies or to discredit it, these efforts are lost once it gets re-shared. The false information then has the potential to reach many new readers who may then develop inaccurate beliefs.

“It is puzzling that sharing of possible misinformation persists in a cohort who are both attuned to and concerned about misinformation and who actively seek the debunking of misinformation. Sharing misinformation, even when it is not done to deceive, increases the chances that it will promote faulty beliefs in others,” Saling and her team say.

Although fact-checking newsletters likely increase people’s vigilance to fake news, it appears they do not necessarily keep people from spreading misinformation. The authors say that, instead, a more explicit intervention may be needed.

“The next studies in our research program involve using experience sampling methodology to ask people to report at multiple time points what information they are sharing, motivations for sharing and platforms on which information is shared, and using eye tracking to investigate how people interact with information in order to determine the optimal way to present debunked information to increase its efficacy,” Saling said.

The study, “No one is immune to misinformation: An investigation of misinformation sharing by subscribers to a fact-checking newsletter”, was authored by Lauren L. Saling, Devi Mallal, Falk Scholer, Russell Skelton, and Damiano Spina.

RELATED

Surprisingly few “#bodypositivity” videos on TikTok actually contain messaging related to body positivity, study finds
Psychopathy

Hybristophilia: Study links TikTok to women’s sexual attraction to criminals

October 6, 2025
This is the devastating, long-term reality for hundreds of thousands after COVID
COVID-19

This is the devastating, long-term reality for hundreds of thousands after COVID

October 2, 2025
The way you blink reveals how music is shaping your attention, new study finds
Political Psychology

Moral tone of right-wing Redditors varies by context, but left-wingers’ tone tends to stay steady

September 30, 2025
Scientists say X (formerly Twitter) has lost its professional edge — and Bluesky is taking its place
Social Media

Scientists say X (formerly Twitter) has lost its professional edge — and Bluesky is taking its place

September 27, 2025
Exposure to smartphone light suppresses melatonin levels at night
Anxiety

Night owls more prone to problematic smartphone use, with loneliness and anxiety as key factors

September 22, 2025
Psychology researchers uncover how personality relates to rejection of negative feedback
Mental Health

Genetic analysis upends narrative linking social media use to lower wellbeing

September 21, 2025
Evolutionary psychology reveals patterns in mass murder motivations across life stages
COVID-19

Study finds video gaming during COVID-19 had little impact on mental health

September 12, 2025
COVID-19 lockdowns linked to lasting disruptions in teen brain and body systems
COVID-19

COVID-19 lockdowns linked to lasting disruptions in teen brain and body systems

September 11, 2025

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Public opinion shifts affect cardiovascular responses during political speech

Internet use is linked to better cognitive health in older adults

White people may dance worse under stereotype threat

A sense of shared power predicts a healthier sex life in married couples

New study finds “superagers” have younger-looking brains over time

Positive porn attitudes linked to better sexual well-being in young women

Taylor Swift’s accent: Scientists trace the evolution of singer’s dialect

People with dark personality traits show less concern for animal rights

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy