Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

LSD enhances learning and exploratory behavior in humans, according to new placebo-controlled research

by Eric W. Dolan
May 6, 2023
in Cognitive Science, LSD
Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

The psychedelic drug lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) makes people learn faster when receiving feedback and enhances exploratory behavior, according to new research published in Psychological Medicine. The findings could help shed light on the underlying cognitive mechanisms behind the potential therapeutic effects of LSD.

LSD-assisted psychotherapy has shown promise in the treatment of conditions such as anxiety, depression, and post-traumatic stress disorder, although it is still illegal in most countries. The drug, when used in a controlled setting under the guidance of a trained therapist, is believed to enhance the patient’s ability to explore and process difficult emotions and experiences, leading to insights and potential breakthroughs in therapy.

“The potential therapeutic effects of LSD may involve new beneficial learning about the world, such as forming new associations,” said study author Jonathan Kanen, a Gates Cambridge Scholar and an incoming psychiatry resident at The Mount Sinai Hospital in New York City.

“An important way by which psychedelic drugs, such as LSD, act is through increasing serotonin signals in a unique way, through stimulating the serotonin 2A receptor — this is known to be involved in learning and flexible thinking. Few studies, however, have examined how LSD affects learning through trial and error in humans.”

Kanen and his colleagues aimed to investigate the effects of LSD on learning and decision-making in humans, with the ultimate goal of identifying the psychological mechanisms by which LSD could have potential therapeutic benefits for mental health. The researchers used a task called probabilistic reversal learning (PRL) to assess how participants made choices under uncertainty and how they adapted their behavior based on feedback.

The study included 19 healthy volunteers who were over the age of 21. Each participant attended two sessions that were at least two weeks apart. In each session, the participant received either LSD or a placebo in a single-blind within-subjects balanced-order design before completing a PRL task. This means that each participant received both LSD and placebo, but the order in which they received them was randomly determined and counterbalanced across participants.

The LSD dose was 75 μg (a moderate dose), which was administered intravenously in 10mL saline solution. The placebo was also administered in 10mL saline solution, but did not contain LSD.

The PRL task involved presenting participants with three visual stimuli and asking them to choose one of them. In the first half of the task, one of the stimuli was associated with positive feedback on 75% of trials, another with positive feedback on 50% of trials, and the third with positive feedback on only 25% of trials. After 40 trials, the probabilities associated with each stimulus were reversed, so the stimulus that was previously correct 75% of the time was now correct only 25% of the time, and vice versa.

The researchers used both classical statistics and computational models of reinforcement learning to analyze the data. Classical statistics allowed them to measure how LSD affected overt choice behavior during the PRL task, while computational models of reinforcement learning allowed them to investigate the underlying learning mechanisms.

Kanen and his colleagues found that LSD increased the speed at which participants updated their expectations based on feedback. This means that the participants who took LSD were quicker to learn from their experiences than those who took a placebo.

Additionally, those who took LSD were more exploratory in their behaviour, meaning that they were more likely to try new options when making decisions. The researchers also found that LSD increased the impact of both positive and negative feedback, but it enhanced learning from reward significantly more than it did from punishment.

“LSD enhanced a fundamental learning process,” Kanen told PsyPost. “When something better than expected happens to us, the brain updates our expectations or beliefs about whether something good might happen again if we do the same thing. LSD boosted how rapidly expectations are updated, particularly after better than expected outcomes. This indicates that LSD may make the brain more changeable. These results could be relevant to its potential beneficial effects for people with certain mental illnesses.”

The findings are line with previous research, which has provided preliminary evidence that LSD might enhance learning and memory by promoting brain plasticity.

By enhancing the rate at which people learn from feedback and increasing exploratory behavior, LSD may help individuals with mental health issues break free from negative thought patterns and develop new, more positive associations.

However, Kanen noted that his study only tested the acute effects of LSD, meaning that it only examined the immediate and short-term effects of the drug. Further research would be needed to investigate the potential long-term effects of LSD on learning and decision-making, and how these effects are related to therapeutic outcomes.

“An open question is whether these effects of LSD on learning help us understand how to optimize treatments involving psychedelic drugs,” he told PsyPost. “We studied learning right after administration of LSD — do these effects on learning and flexibility persist after the drug wears off? Whether these effects of LSD generalize to other psychedelic drugs such as psilocybin remains to be tested.”

The study, “Effect of lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) on reinforcement learning in humans“, was authored by Jonathan W. Kanen, Qiang Luo, Mojtaba Rostami Kandroodi, Rudolf N. Cardinal, Trevor W. Robbins, David J. Nutt, Robin L. Carhart-Harris, and Hanneke E. M. den Ouden.

TweetSendScanShareSendPin4ShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Scientists find genetic basis for how much people enjoy music
Cognitive Science

Is humor inherited? Twin study suggests the ability to be funny may not run in the family

July 10, 2025

A first-of-its-kind study set out to discover whether being funny is something you inherit. By testing twins on their joke-making skills, researchers found that your sense of humor might have less to do with DNA than you'd think.

Read moreDetails
Even in healthy adults, high blood sugar levels are linked to impaired brain function
Memory

Neuroscientists decode how people juggle multiple items in working memory

July 8, 2025

New neuroscience research shows how the brain decides which memories deserve more attention. By tracking brain activity, scientists found that the frontal cortex helps direct limited memory resources, allowing people to remember high-priority information more precisely than less relevant details.

Read moreDetails
New study uncovers a surprising effect of cold-water immersion
Cognitive Science

New study uncovers a surprising effect of cold-water immersion

July 8, 2025

Cold-water immersion increases energy expenditure—but it may also drive people to eat more afterward. A study in Physiology & Behavior found participants consumed significantly more food following cold exposure, possibly due to internal cooling effects that continue after leaving the water.

Read moreDetails
Positive attitudes toward AI linked to problematic social media use
Cognitive Science

People with higher cognitive ability have weaker moral foundations, new study finds

July 7, 2025

A large study has found that individuals with greater cognitive ability are less likely to endorse moral values such as compassion, fairness, loyalty, and purity. The results point to a consistent negative relationship between intelligence and moral intuitions.

Read moreDetails
These common sounds can impair your learning, according to new psychology research
Cognitive Science

These common sounds can impair your learning, according to new psychology research

July 4, 2025

Your brain’s ancient defense system might be sabotaging your test scores. New research suggests our "behavioral immune system," which makes us subconsciously alert to signs of illness, can be triggered by coughs and sniffles.

Read moreDetails
From fireflies to brain cells: Unraveling the complex web of synchrony in networks
Addiction

Understanding “neuronal ensembles” could revolutionize addiction treatment

July 3, 2025

The same brain system that rewards you for a delicious meal is hijacked by drugs like fentanyl. A behavioral neuroscientist explains how understanding the specific memories behind these rewards is the key to treating addiction without harming our essential survival instincts.

Read moreDetails
Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time
Memory

Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time

July 3, 2025

Our perception of time is more fragile than we think. Scientists have uncovered a powerful illusion where repeated exposure to information makes us misremember it as happening much further in the past, significantly distorting our mental timelines.

Read moreDetails
Peppermint tea boosts memory and attention—but why?
Cognitive Science

Peppermint tea boosts memory and attention—but why?

July 2, 2025

Can a cup of peppermint tea sharpen your mind? A new study suggests it can—but not in the way scientists expected. Improved memory and attention followed the tea, but increased brain blood flow wasn't the reason why.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Frequent egg consumption linked to lower risk of Alzheimer’s dementia, study finds

Psychopathic personality and weak impulse control pair up to predict teen property crime

Low sexual activity, body shape, and mood may combine in ways that shorten lives, new study suggests

Highly irritable teens are more likely to bully others, but anxiety mitigates this tendency

Neuroscientists identify brain pathway that prioritizes safety over other needs

Liberals and conservatives live differently — but people think the divide is even bigger than it is

Neuroscientists shed new light on how heroin disrupts prefrontal brain function

New research identifies four distinct health pathways linked to Alzheimer’s disease

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy