Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Conspiracy Theories

New research supports the “rabbit hole” model of conspiratorial thinking

by Eric W. Dolan
August 8, 2025
in Conspiracy Theories
[Adobe Stock]

[Adobe Stock]

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

A new study published in the European Journal of Social Psychology provides evidence that believing in one conspiracy theory can slightly increase the likelihood of believing in others over time. While the effects were small, the findings support a long-standing theory that some people may develop interconnected systems of conspiratorial thinking, where one belief helps reinforce others.

Although previous studies had shown that people who believe in one conspiracy theory tend to believe in others, there was little direct evidence about how these beliefs develop over time. One influential theory, proposed by psychologist Ted Goertzel in 1994, suggested that conspiracy beliefs could form a closed system, where each belief supports and amplifies the others.

This model has been widely cited, but its central claim—that believing in one conspiracy theory increases the likelihood of adopting others—had not been tested in a way that could establish causality. To address this gap, a group of researchers from institutions in New Zealand, Australia, the United Kingdom, and Germany conducted two extensive longitudinal studies to examine how beliefs in various conspiracy theories influence each other over time.

“There’s been a lot of research showing that people who believe one conspiracy theory are more likely than average to believe other conspiracy theories. A well-known theory suggests this is because developing a belief in one conspiracy theory causes people to develop beliefs in others,” said study author Matt N. Williams, an associate professor at the School of Psychology at Massey University.

“This is the popular idea of ‘going down the rabbit hole.’ But despite a lot of discussion about this idea in the academic literature, there haven’t previously been any studies on the topic with a credible way of testing causal effects. I realized that by doing longitudinal studies it’d be possible to test whether people who develop a belief in one conspiracy theory are more likely to subsequently develop beliefs in other conspiracy theories.”

In the first study, the team followed 498 participants from Australia and New Zealand across seven monthly survey waves. Participants answered questions about their agreement with 10 well-known conspiracy theories, such as beliefs that the September 11 attacks were an inside job, that fluoride is added to water to control minds, and that COVID-19 vaccines contain microchips. Responses were given on a five-point scale ranging from strong disagreement to strong agreement. Most participants disagreed with most of the theories, but there was still enough variation in responses over time to examine patterns of change.

The researchers used a statistical approach known as a random intercept cross-lagged panel model, or RI-CLPM. This method allowed them to isolate changes within individuals over time, separating those from stable personality traits that might predispose someone to conspiracy beliefs in general. They then analyzed whether an increase in belief in one conspiracy theory in a given month was followed by increases in belief in other theories the next month.

The results showed that on average, small increases in belief in one conspiracy theory were followed by small increases in belief in others. The individual effects were tiny—typically a few hundredths of a point on a five-point scale—but consistent across the data. While none of the individual cross-lagged effects were statistically significant after correcting for multiple comparisons, the average effect across all 90 possible pairings was positive and significant. This pattern supported the idea that, for at least some people, adopting one conspiratorial belief can make it more likely they’ll adopt others in the future.

To confirm the results and strengthen the causal interpretation, the researchers conducted a second, preregistered study. This study included a larger and more diverse sample of 980 participants from the United Kingdom, Australia, and New Zealand. The data were collected over 13 consecutive months, and the survey included 11 conspiracy theories—some overlapping with those in the first study and some newly added. These included beliefs that climate change is a hoax, that genetically modified foods are dangerous and being covered up, and that the COVID-19 pandemic was used as a pretext to restrict civil liberties.

Again, the researchers used RI-CLPM to analyze the data. They preregistered their hypothesis that belief in any one conspiracy theory would positively influence belief in all others over time and laid out strict criteria for what would count as support or refutation of that hypothesis.

According to their preregistered plan, even a single negative and statistically significant cross-lagged effect could have falsified their theory. But in the results, none of the effects were significantly negative. One positive effect remained statistically significant even after a strict correction for multiple comparisons, and the average cross-lagged effect was again small but positive and statistically significant.

“The results suggest that developing a belief in one conspiracy theory probably does make you more likely to subsequently develop beliefs in other conspiracy theories, but only very slightly!” Williams told PsyPost. “The effect sizes we found were statistically significant but very small.”

“This lines up with one of our previous studies showing that people’s levels of belief (or disbelief) in conspiracy theories tend to be quite stable over time. In that paper, we concluded: ‘People developing beliefs in a succession of conspiracy theories are often characterised as falling down a ‘rabbit hole’—a reference to Lewis Carroll’s Alice’s Adventures in Wonderland. It seems that if conspiracy theorists do fall down a rabbit hole, it is typically one with a rather gradual slope.'”

As with all research, there are limitations to note. One concern is that the findings could be influenced by factors that change over time and affect belief in multiple theories, such as exposure to new information or political events. These time-varying influences were not controlled in the analyses, meaning they could potentially explain some of the observed effects.

“Our paper was specifically focused on estimating causal effects,” Williams noted. “We did collect longitudinal data, and applied a form of analysis that can rule out one type of confounding variable—confounding variables that are stable over time. However, our studies definitely aren’t as conclusive as a true experiment would be, and it is possible that our estimates of causal effects could be biased (upwards or downwards!)”

“It’s also worth mentioning that the two studies in this paper are about the effects of beliefs in different conspiracy theories on one another. They weren’t designed to tell us anything about other consequences of belief in conspiracy theories. That’s a different question, and other researchers have published on this topic. But to me, the fact that people are willing to entertain conspiracy theories isn’t a bad thing in and of itself; real conspiracies do happen! That said, when beliefs about conspiracies become unmoored from reality, negative consequences can take place.”

Another limitation is that the surveys may not accurately reflect participants’ true beliefs. Factors such as social desirability bias, inattentiveness, or insincere responding could have distorted how people reported their agreement with the conspiracy theories.

“People sometimes claim to believe a conspiracy theory in a survey when they don’t really believe it (e.g., because they’re making a joke or trying to troll us),” Williams explained. “Such insincere responses could possibly distort some of our findings in this research area. For example, it could inflate estimates of the prevalence of beliefs in conspiracy theories. Some of my collaborators and I are therefore working on a study where we aim to find out more about insincere responding and how it affects research on conspiracy theories.”

“I think it’s important that research on conspiracy theories (or any topic, really) is done in a transparent fashion. So we shared open data and materials along with this paper, so readers can check our analyses and replicate our work. We also preregistered Study 2, so that readers can have more confidence that we didn’t ‘cherry pick’ our findings. The paper itself is also open access.”

The study, “Does Developing a Belief in One Conspiracy Theory Lead a Person to be More Likely to Believe in Others?,” was authored by Matt N. Williams, Mathew D. Marques, John R. Kerr, Stephen R. Hill, Mathew Ling, and Edward J. R. Clarke.

RELATED

Social class shapes perceptions of societal contribution
Conspiracy Theories

Worsening economic conditions fuel anti-immigrant conspiracy beliefs and support for violence

August 7, 2025

Belief in anti-immigrant conspiracies may be fueled by financial hardship and a sense of societal breakdown, according to new research. Across six studies, economic stress predicted support for discriminatory policies and even violent actions against non-European immigrants.

Read moreDetails
Conspiracy believers tend to overrate their cognitive abilities and think most others agree with them
Conspiracy Theories

Conspiracy believers tend to overrate their cognitive abilities and think most others agree with them

June 16, 2025

People who believe in conspiracy theories tend to overestimate their own abilities and wrongly assume that others share their views, according to a new study. The findings highlight overconfidence as a key factor behind conspiracy belief.

Read moreDetails
AI-powered study sheds light on how QAnon beliefs shatter family bonds
Conspiracy Theories

AI-powered study sheds light on how QAnon beliefs shatter family bonds

June 1, 2025

A new study used artificial intelligence to analyze over 75,000 Reddit posts and uncover the deep emotional toll QAnon beliefs take on families. The findings reveal widespread grief, fear, and strained relationships as loved ones struggle with ideological division.

Read moreDetails
New study flips the script on conspiracy beliefs and economic insecurity
Conspiracy Theories

New research highlights spite as a motivator of conspiracy theory beliefs

May 30, 2025

Psychologists have found that conspiracy theory beliefs may stem from spite triggered by feelings of social, existential, or cognitive disadvantage. The findings suggest that tackling misinformation requires addressing deeper social inequalities and psychological frustrations.

Read moreDetails
Delusion-like cognitive biases predict conspiracy theory belief
Cognitive Science

Delusion-like cognitive biases predict conspiracy theory belief

May 28, 2025

People prone to conspiracy theories may share cognitive tendencies with those who experience delusional thinking. Two new studies suggest that biases like anomalous perception and impulsive reasoning help explain why some are more likely to embrace conspiratorial beliefs.

Read moreDetails
People with a higher conspiracy mentality have a general tendency to judge others as untrustworthy
Authoritarianism

Authoritarian minds may be primed for conspiracy beliefs, study suggests

April 21, 2025

Contrary to popular belief, conspiracy theories may not drive democratic decline—rather, those who reject democracy seem more prone to conspiratorial thinking.

Read moreDetails
Measles is back and anti-vaccine sentiment is to blame
Conspiracy Theories

Measles is back and anti-vaccine sentiment is to blame

April 18, 2025

Measles is surging again, and experts say fear, mistrust, and misinformation—not just ignorance—are fueling vaccine hesitancy.

Read moreDetails
Sleep-deprived minds are more likely to believe in conspiracy theories
Conspiracy Theories

Sleep-deprived minds are more likely to believe in conspiracy theories

April 17, 2025

A new study suggests that how well you sleep may influence how much you believe in conspiracy theories.

Read moreDetails

Leave a Reply Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Only premium subscribers can comment — log in or join now.

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Ketamine’s paradox: Life-changing therapy for some, devastating addiction for others

Childhood trauma tied to sexual narcissism and hypersexual behavior

Populism may act as a “thermometer” for democratic health

Melatonin supplements may boost DNA repair in night shift workers, new study finds

Bright children from low-income homes lose cognitive edge in early secondary school

New research supports the “rabbit hole” model of conspiratorial thinking

Moderate aerobic exercise enhances the brain’s “eighth sense”

Scientists have uncovered these weird facts about psychopathic individuals

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy