Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology

A key psychological factor helps explain how planetarium films boost interest in science

by Eric W. Dolan
February 15, 2025
in Social Psychology
[Adobe Stock]

[Adobe Stock]

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

Getting people excited about science can be a challenge, but a recent study offers a promising approach. Researchers discovered that inducing awe, specifically through immersive planetarium films, is highly effective at sparking and growing people’s interest in scientific subjects. This suggests awe could be key to making science more engaging for everyone. The work has been published in the journal Cognition and Emotion.

Scientists have long recognized that emotions play a significant role in how we learn and remember things. Emotions related to knowledge and understanding, termed epistemic emotions, are particularly relevant in learning environments. For example, feeling curious can lead to deeper engagement with new material, while boredom can hinder learning. Awe is one such emotion, often described as the feeling we get when we encounter something vast and beyond our current understanding, prompting us to adjust our perspective of the world. Though awe is thought to be a driving force behind science learning, exactly how it affects our thinking processes during learning has remained largely unknown.

Previous research exploring awe has often taken place in controlled laboratory settings, raising questions about whether these findings truly reflect real-world experiences. To address this gap, a team of researchers decided to investigate awe in a more natural environment: a science center planetarium. They aimed to understand if the awe experienced while watching planetarium films, designed to be immersive and awe-inspiring, could enhance learning and cultivate a greater interest in science-related topics. Specifically, they wanted to examine how this situational awe impacts the ability to remember information from the films and how it influences a person’s interest in the film’s subject matter.

“I became interested in researching the various effects of awe on human cognition, behavior, and decision-making because the research literature suggested that awe is a very powerful emotion yet largely understudied,” said study author Oksana Kanerva, a lecturer at University of Helsinki who conducted the study while a senior researcher at the University of Turku.

To conduct their investigation, the researchers recruited 131 adult visitors to a science center who were planning to watch one of the planetarium’s films. Before entering the planetarium, participants answered questions about their existing knowledge of the film’s topic and their initial interest in it. They also provided some basic background information about themselves. The science center was showing three different films at the time: one about Earth’s climate system, one about the Northern Lights, and one about stars and planets in the distant universe. Participants watched whichever film they had chosen to see as regular science center visitors.

Immediately after watching their chosen film, participants completed a series of questionnaires. They again rated their interest in the film’s topic to see if it had changed after viewing. They also completed questionnaires designed to measure different emotions they might have experienced, including awe, surprise, curiosity, enjoyment, confusion, anxiety, and boredom. To specifically measure awe, the researchers used a questionnaire that asked participants to rate how strongly they felt various sensations during the film, such as feeling connected to everything around them, feeling a sense of physical smallness, or experiencing goosebumps. Additionally, participants completed a questionnaire assessing their general tendency to think critically.

Finally, participants completed a recognition memory task to assess their recall of the film’s content. This task presented participants with a series of statements related to the film they had just watched. Some of these statements were phrases taken directly from the film. Others were slightly altered versions of phrases from the film, containing minor factual errors. Some statements were completely nonsensical, contradicting basic scientific knowledge. Lastly, some statements were inferences – ideas that could be logically drawn from the film’s information but were not explicitly stated. For each statement, participants had to decide whether they believed it had been presented in the film. This recognition task was designed to reveal how awe might influence different types of memory processes, such as accurately remembering details versus making logical connections or falling for misleading information.

The study’s results confirmed that planetarium films do indeed evoke awe in viewers. Participants reported experiencing awe while watching the films, although the intensity of this emotion varied somewhat depending on the specific film. Interestingly, the film about the Northern Lights seemed to elicit a stronger sense of awe compared to the film about stars. The researchers noted that the Northern Lights film had longer sentences and perhaps presented more complex information, which might have contributed to the stronger awe response. They also found that individuals who had a greater inclination towards critical thinking reported feeling more connected to their surroundings during the awe experience.

In terms of emotions accompanying awe, the study revealed that awe was strongly associated with surprise. While awe was also linked to other emotions like curiosity and enjoyment, surprise was the most significant emotion that consistently coincided with feelings of awe. This suggests that the unexpected or surprising nature of the planetarium film content may be a key ingredient in triggering awe.

When examining the recognition memory task, the researchers found that experiencing higher levels of awe was linked to decreased accuracy in identifying errors, nonsense statements, and inferences. In other words, people who felt more awe were more likely to mistakenly say that these incorrect or inferred statements had actually been presented in the film. This suggests that awe, in this context, did not enhance memory accuracy. Interestingly, prior knowledge of the film’s topic played a role in memory performance. People with more prior knowledge were better at spotting errors and nonsense statements but were more prone to falsely recognize inference statements as being from the film.

“Based on the recognition memory task results, visitors experiencing higher awe tended to give more affirmative answers to questions where ‘no’ was the correct response, compared to those who experienced less awe,” Kanerva told PsyPost. “This suggests that awe may promote confirmation bias. More research on the possible connection between awe and confirmation bias is needed.”

But perhaps the most important finding was the impact of awe on interest. The study clearly demonstrated that experiencing awe while watching the planetarium films significantly increased participants’ interest in the topics covered in the films. This boost in interest was particularly noticeable for those who reported feeling higher levels of awe. This finding supports the idea that awe can serve as a catalyst, sparking curiosity and driving individuals to become more interested in science-related subjects.

“This is one of the first studies to document an increase in topic-specific interest in a science museum exhibition, specifically in the topic of planetarium films,” Kanerva said. “The key takeaway is that awe has the potential to make people more interested in science-related topics.”

The researchers acknowledged some limitations to their study. Because the study observed people in a real-world setting, it could only show associations, not definitively prove that awe directly causes increased interest or altered memory. Future research could build upon these findings by refining the memory tasks to be more challenging, perhaps by testing memory after a longer delay. Experimentally manipulating awe, for instance by comparing awe-inducing films to more neutral ones, could help to establish a clearer causal link between awe and science interest.

“The long-term goals are to investigate the effects of awe on human behavior and decision-making in natural settings, extending beyond science museums,” Kanerva said.

The study, “Impact of awe on topic interest and recognition memory for information in planetarium films,” was authored by Oksana Kanerva, Tuomo Häikiö, Helmi Päällysaho, and Johanna K. Kaakinen.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Gynandromorph research offers insight into the complexities of male sexual attraction
Sexism

Eye-tracking study shows people fixate longer on female aggressors than male ones

June 23, 2025

People pay more visual attention to female aggressors than male ones, but do not consistently judge their actions as more intentional or blameworthy, suggesting that female aggression is seen as unexpected rather than more morally significant.

Read moreDetails
Breakups can trigger trauma in emerging adults
Relationships and Sexual Health

Romantic breakups follow a two-stage decline that begins years before the split, study finds

June 23, 2025

A new study shows that romantic relationships often begin to unravel one to two years before they officially end. Researchers found a two-phase pattern of decline in satisfaction that could help identify when relationships are heading for a breakup.

Read moreDetails
It’s not digital illiteracy: Here’s why older adults are drawn to dubious news
Social Media

Believing “news will find me” is linked to sharing fake news, study finds

June 22, 2025

People who rely on social media to “stumble upon” news are more prone to spreading misinformation, according to a new longitudinal study.

Read moreDetails
Political ambivalence has a surprising relationship with support for violence
Authoritarianism

New study sheds light on the psychological roots of collective violence

June 21, 2025

A new study from Lebanon finds that people with authoritarian beliefs tend to oppose violence against political leaders, while those high in social dominance orientation are more likely to support violence against rival group members.

Read moreDetails
Scientists observe reduced emotional distress in children living near greenery
Racism and Discrimination

Adults’ beliefs about children and race shift when a child’s race is specified, study finds

June 21, 2025

Specifying a child’s race alters how adults perceive their awareness of race and racism, according to new research. Black children are viewed as less “color-evasive” and more racially aware at earlier ages than White children or generic “children.”

Read moreDetails
Study helps untangle the complicated relationship between psychopathy and emotional awareness
Psychopathy

Psychopathic individuals recognize unfairness but are less likely to punish it

June 21, 2025

A new study shows that individuals with higher psychopathic traits are less likely to punish unfair behavior, especially when it costs them personally. The research suggests self-interest, not a lack of moral understanding, drives their reluctance to enforce social norms.

Read moreDetails
The neuroscience of why we cry happy tears
Social Psychology

The neuroscience of why we cry happy tears

June 20, 2025

Why do people cry happy tears? Neuroscience suggests these emotional outbursts occur when the brain becomes overwhelmed by joy, nostalgia, or relief. Far from being irrational, crying during joyful moments helps restore balance and deepen human connection.

Read moreDetails
Epistemic mistrust and dogmatism predict preference for authoritarian-looking leaders
Authoritarianism

Epistemic mistrust and dogmatism predict preference for authoritarian-looking leaders

June 20, 2025

A new study suggests that the way people learn to trust others early in life can shape their political ideology and preference for strong, dominant leaders—though not directly, but through dogmatic thinking and broader political attitudes.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Exposure to heavy metals is associated with higher likelihood of ADHD diagnosis

Eye-tracking study shows people fixate longer on female aggressors than male ones

Romantic breakups follow a two-stage decline that begins years before the split, study finds

Believing “news will find me” is linked to sharing fake news, study finds

A common parasite not only invades the brain — it can also decapitate human sperm

Almost all unmarried pregant women say that the fetus resembles the father, study finds

New neuroscience research reveals brain antioxidant deficit in depression

Scientists uncover kidney-to-brain route for Parkinson’s-related protein spread

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy