Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Mental Health

Alexia: What happens when a brain injury makes you forget how to read

by The Conversation
July 18, 2015
in Mental Health
Photo credit: CollegeDegrees360

Photo credit: CollegeDegrees360

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

Once we have successfully learned how to read, it continues to be easy for most of us. But for some people it can be an immense challenge. In developmental dyslexia, the process of learning to read is disrupted, while in alexia – or acquired dyslexia – brain damage can affect reading ability in previously literate adults.

Patients with pure alexia lose the ability to read fluently following injury to areas in the rear part of the left hemisphere of their brain. The curious thing is that they can still walk, talk, think, and even write like they did before their injury. They just can’t read. Not even what they have written themselves.

Some patients lose the ability to recognise letters and words completely, but more commonly, patients with pure alexia can recognise single letters and will spell their way through words to identify them. As a result, some researchers prefer the term “letter-by-letter reading” to pure alexia.

Pure alexia as a syndrome was first described more than 120 years ago, but researchers still disagree on the cause of the reading problems. They agree that a lesion in the brain causes the problems, but they can’t agree on which cognitive mechanisms may be responsible, or even how the disorder should be defined.

Not a language problem

Evidence from functional brain imaging has led to the idea of a brain area that is specialised in recognising words and letters, called the “visual word form area”. It is this area that is commonly damaged in pure alexia. However, the role of this area in the way we read is highly debated and there is disagreement about whether it is reading-specific, or important for all sorts of visual recognition, such as looking at images or even faces. The same questions are discussed regarding pure alexia: whether the disorder is specific to reading or a more general deficit in somebody’s visual processing ability.

The image shows the lesions of four patients with pure alexia. The colours represent the number of patients with lesions in different areas. The visual word form area is marked by the red crosshairs. (Randi Starrfelt, Cerebal Cortex)

 

In most, if not all cases of pure alexia, other visual perceptual functions such as recognition of numbers or objects are affected, while other language functions, like speech comprehension and production – as well as writing – may be intact.

So it makes sense to look at pure alexia as a visual disorder; if it was a language problem, we would at least expect writing to also be affected, and it’s not. It is also clear, however, that the deficit in pure alexia patients primarily affects recognition of complex visual stimuli. This is because patients with this disorder may perform normally in perceiving simple patterns.

Pure alexic patients have difficulty recognising numbers as well as letters, and also show problems in perceiving more than a few letters or numbers at the same time. So it seems that patterns must be either visually complex, or need to be linked with meaning – such as words – for pure alexic patients to be impaired. On this basis, we have suggested that the core problem for pure alexic patients, is that they see “too little too late” to be able to read fluently.

As you read the words in this article, you need to perceive and integrate multiple letters at a time to access the meaning of the words and the text. Very few other visual tasks demand the same speed and span of apprehension for successful recognition, which is why patients with pure alexia rarely complain of any problems other than in reading.

When letters come easier than words

For normal readers, integrating letters into words is a very simple task that we perform automatically and effortlessly. It may actually be more difficult to focus on a single letter within a word than the word itself.

This is also known as the “word superiority effect” – that people are better at identifying words than single letters, even though words consist of letters that must be processed for the word to be recognised. This effect probably arises because of two things: first, normal readers can process letters in parallel by identifying multiple letters at a time and second, our knowledge of word meaning and word spelling helps us to identify the word.

The word superiority effect is not present in pure alexic patients: they actually perform better recognising single letters than with words. For instance, when they are asked to recognise something that is presented to them for a very short time they would recognise the letters, rather than the word itself. Perhaps it’s no wonder that many of them resort to letter-by-letter reading.

In evolutionary terms, reading is a very recent skill which takes time and instruction to learn. If a dedicated brain area is responsible for visual recognition of words then this function of the brain must have been created in each of us as we learn, rather than through evolutionary mechanisms and development.

But although the “visual word form area” may be specialised for reading, and this specialisation is created through learning to read, the area itself is not new – the brain hasn’t grown in any way. That is one of the intriguing things about the brain: even if all we learn is stored in there, the brain doesn’t grow much bigger when we learn. Instead, it seems to be reorganised, so that new skills may relocate or at least slightly displace older skills.

This has been referred to as “neuronal recycling” by the French neuroscientist Stanislas Dehaene, the man who also coined the term “the visual word form area”. It seems that the visual word form area, in addition to being crucial for visual word recognition, continues to contribute to our recognition of other visual stimuli such as images of objects. Exploring this relationship between reading and other cognitive skills is a new avenue in research on reading and the brain where there is still much to learn.

The Conversation

By Randi Starrfelt, University of Copenhagen

Randi Starrfelt is Associate Professor, Department of Psychology at University of Copenhagen.

This article was originally published on The Conversation.
Read the original article.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

New research sheds light on psychedelics’ complex relationship to psychosis and mania
Mental Health

Psychedelic retreats show promise in easing depression, PTSD, and reintegration struggles among veterans

July 16, 2025

Military veterans who attended psilocybin or ayahuasca retreats showed improvements in measures of depression, PTSD, anxiety, and reintegration into civilian life compared to their baseline assessments.

Read moreDetails
Infant brain microstructure may predict emotional development
Autism

Neurons in an autism model fail to distinguish social from non-social touch

July 16, 2025

New research suggests "neural confusion" may underlie social avoidance in some forms of autism. In a mouse study, neurons that should value social contact responded similarly to both another mouse and a plastic object, blurring the line between the two experiences.

Read moreDetails
Superagers’ brains show superior white matter health, study finds
Addiction

Scientists identify the brain’s built-in brake for binge drinking

July 15, 2025

In a breakthrough for addiction science, neurobiologists have pinpointed the precise brain circuit that suppresses the urge to binge drink. This discovery of the brain's own "off switch" could revolutionize how we understand and treat alcohol use disorder.

Read moreDetails
Borderline personality and depression: New findings show gender differences in adolescents
Early Life Adversity and Childhood Maltreatment

Childhood maltreatment linked to emotion regulation difficulties and teen mental health problems

July 15, 2025

A longitudinal study in China suggests that teens who were maltreated as children often use maladaptive emotion regulation strategies, which may help explain their heightened risk for depression and anxiety symptoms later in adolescence.

Read moreDetails
Caffeine use prevents stress-induced impairment of spatial memory
Caffeine

Caffeine may help prevent depression-like symptoms by protecting the gut-brain connection

July 15, 2025

Caffeine may help prevent stress-induced depression-like symptoms in mice by protecting gut health and reducing inflammation. While more research is needed, the findings raise the possibility that everyday dietary habits could play a meaningful role in mental health

Read moreDetails
People do not necessarily become happier at older age, study finds
Depression

The triglyceride-glucose index: Can it predict depression risk in the elderly?

July 14, 2025

Older adults with higher triglyceride-glucose levels were not more likely to develop depression over time after accounting for health and lifestyle factors, suggesting this metabolic marker does not predict future depression in this age group.

Read moreDetails
First human study sheds light on the cognitive and physiological effects of 3-MMC
ADHD

People with ADHD exhibit altered brain activity before making high-stakes choices

July 14, 2025

New research reveals that the brains of adults with ADHD function differently when anticipating risky decisions. They show reduced activation in key areas for self-awareness and emotional integration, offering a neurological basis for challenges with impulsivity and decision-making.

Read moreDetails
New research shows the psychological toll of the 2024 presidential election
Anxiety

New research shows the psychological toll of the 2024 presidential election

July 13, 2025

Among young adults, stress from election news was linked to higher risks of depression and anxiety, while pre-election anticipatory stress was linked to depression only. Stress about the election outcome was not associated with either condition.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Women favor men with attractive faces when making social bargaining decisions

Caffeine increases brain complexity during sleep, study shows

Psychedelic retreats show promise in easing depression, PTSD, and reintegration struggles among veterans

Neurons in an autism model fail to distinguish social from non-social touch

Medicinal cannabis may actually worsen sleep, a new study finds

Scientists identify the brain’s built-in brake for binge drinking

Trump’s speeches stump AI: Study reveals ChatGPT’s struggle with metaphors

Childhood maltreatment linked to emotion regulation difficulties and teen mental health problems

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy