Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

Astronauts surprisingly good at judging distances without gravity

by Eric W. Dolan
March 30, 2024
in Cognitive Science
Expedition 68 Flight Engineer Koichi Wakata of the Japan Aerospace Exploration Agency (JAXA) is pictured in his Extravehicular Mobility Unit, or spacesuit, during his second spacewalk. (Credit: NASA)

Expedition 68 Flight Engineer Koichi Wakata of the Japan Aerospace Exploration Agency (JAXA) is pictured in his Extravehicular Mobility Unit, or spacesuit, during his second spacewalk. (Credit: NASA)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

Astronauts possess an uncanny knack for judging distances in the weightlessness of space, according to new research published in the journal npj Microgravity. The findings revealed that astronauts possess a surprisingly accurate ability to estimate distances traveled in a microgravity environment, such as aboard the International Space Station (ISS).

The research was a collaborative effort spearheaded by York University in partnership with the Canadian Space Agency, NASA, and other international space agencies. But why embark on such a study? The venture into space poses unique challenges to astronauts, among them the absence of gravity.

This lack of gravity affects various bodily functions and senses, including how we perceive movement and orient ourselves in space. Given the critical nature of precise movement and navigation in spacecraft or while conducting spacewalks, understanding and improving astronauts’ spatial awareness in microgravity is paramount.

“It has been repeatedly shown that the perception of gravity influences perceptual skill. The most profound way of looking at the influence of gravity is to take it away, which is why we took our research into space,” said study author Laurence Harris, a psychology professor and director of the Multisensory Integration Lab.

“We’ve had a steady presence for close to a quarter century in space and with space efforts only increasing as we plan to go back to the moon and beyond, answering health-and-safety questions only becomes more important. Based on our findings it seems as though humans are surprisingly able to compensate adequately for the lack of an Earth-normal environment using vision.”

The research focused on two main groups: astronauts who would perform the tasks both on Earth and in the microgravity environment of the ISS, and a control group that underwent similar testing solely on Earth. The astronaut group comprised 15 individuals (8 women and 7 men), though not all completed the study due to various reasons such as delays in their space flight or the inability to complete the second test session within a specific timeframe.

This left 12 astronauts (6 men and 6 women) who fully completed all aspects of the testing protocol. The control group consisted of 22 participants initially, with 20 completing the study due to dropout caused by motion sickness in some cases.

The researchers used virtual reality (VR) technology to simulate a three-dimensional hallway, which served as the primary setting for the experiments. The hallway was designed with light spots on the walls to create optic flow, mimicking the visual cues one would experience while moving through space.

The experimental procedure was carefully crafted to assess the participants’ ability to estimate distances in a simulated environment. Participants viewed a target at various distances in the virtual hallway and were asked to estimate the egocentric distance to the target. Once they had made their estimation, they signaled the start of the trial, and the target disappeared, replaced by optic flow simulating movement towards the target’s location.

Participants then indicated when they felt they had reached the target’s position. This process was repeated across multiple trials and distances, without providing any feedback on performance to ensure that learning effects did not influence the results.

The astronauts underwent testing at five different stages: before their spaceflight, early and late during their mission on the ISS, and early and late after their return to Earth. While on Earth, tests were conducted with astronauts in sitting and supine (lying face up) postures to simulate different orientations relative to gravity.

A key discovery of the research was the significant role that visual cues, or optic flow, play in astronauts’ perception of movement and distance in space. Optic flow refers to the pattern of apparent motion of objects, surfaces, and edges in a visual scene caused by the relative motion between an observer and the scene. In the weightlessness of space, where the vestibular cues related to gravity are not available, astronauts seem to rely more heavily on these visual cues to gauge how far they have traveled.

The study also found that posture affects perception of distance on Earth. Specifically, when participants were in a supine position (lying face up), they tended to estimate distances as being shorter compared to when they were sitting upright. This finding was consistent with the hypothesis that the absence of typical gravitational cues might lead to a greater reliance on visual information for estimating distances.

Furthermore, the results showed no significant difference in the astronauts’ performance before and after their spaceflight. This indicates that the experience of microgravity does not adversely affect the astronauts’ ability to perceive distances upon returning to Earth. It’s an encouraging sign for long-duration space missions, suggesting that astronauts can quickly re-adjust to Earth’s gravity without lasting impairments in spatial perception.

However, the study has its limitations, including the small sample size and the exclusion of data from participants who did not complete all test sessions. Furthermore, the astronauts were not tested immediately upon reaching the ISS, which means the initial adaptation phase to microgravity was not captured.

The researchers call for further studies to explore these findings, especially to understand the long-term effects of spaceflight on spatial orientation and how these insights could benefit people with balance disorders on Earth.

The study, “The effects of long-term exposure to microgravity and body orientation relative to gravity on perceived traveled distance,” was authored by Björn Jörges, Nils Bury, Meaghan McManus, Ambika Bansal, Robert S. Allison, Michael Jenkin, and Laurence R. Harris.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Scientists find genetic basis for how much people enjoy music
Cognitive Science

Is humor inherited? Twin study suggests the ability to be funny may not run in the family

July 10, 2025

A first-of-its-kind study set out to discover whether being funny is something you inherit. By testing twins on their joke-making skills, researchers found that your sense of humor might have less to do with DNA than you'd think.

Read moreDetails
Even in healthy adults, high blood sugar levels are linked to impaired brain function
Memory

Neuroscientists decode how people juggle multiple items in working memory

July 8, 2025

New neuroscience research shows how the brain decides which memories deserve more attention. By tracking brain activity, scientists found that the frontal cortex helps direct limited memory resources, allowing people to remember high-priority information more precisely than less relevant details.

Read moreDetails
New study uncovers a surprising effect of cold-water immersion
Cognitive Science

New study uncovers a surprising effect of cold-water immersion

July 8, 2025

Cold-water immersion increases energy expenditure—but it may also drive people to eat more afterward. A study in Physiology & Behavior found participants consumed significantly more food following cold exposure, possibly due to internal cooling effects that continue after leaving the water.

Read moreDetails
Positive attitudes toward AI linked to problematic social media use
Cognitive Science

People with higher cognitive ability have weaker moral foundations, new study finds

July 7, 2025

A large study has found that individuals with greater cognitive ability are less likely to endorse moral values such as compassion, fairness, loyalty, and purity. The results point to a consistent negative relationship between intelligence and moral intuitions.

Read moreDetails
These common sounds can impair your learning, according to new psychology research
Cognitive Science

These common sounds can impair your learning, according to new psychology research

July 4, 2025

Your brain’s ancient defense system might be sabotaging your test scores. New research suggests our "behavioral immune system," which makes us subconsciously alert to signs of illness, can be triggered by coughs and sniffles.

Read moreDetails
From fireflies to brain cells: Unraveling the complex web of synchrony in networks
Addiction

Understanding “neuronal ensembles” could revolutionize addiction treatment

July 3, 2025

The same brain system that rewards you for a delicious meal is hijacked by drugs like fentanyl. A behavioral neuroscientist explains how understanding the specific memories behind these rewards is the key to treating addiction without harming our essential survival instincts.

Read moreDetails
Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time
Memory

Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time

July 3, 2025

Our perception of time is more fragile than we think. Scientists have uncovered a powerful illusion where repeated exposure to information makes us misremember it as happening much further in the past, significantly distorting our mental timelines.

Read moreDetails
Peppermint tea boosts memory and attention—but why?
Cognitive Science

Peppermint tea boosts memory and attention—but why?

July 2, 2025

Can a cup of peppermint tea sharpen your mind? A new study suggests it can—but not in the way scientists expected. Improved memory and attention followed the tea, but increased brain blood flow wasn't the reason why.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Frequent egg consumption linked to lower risk of Alzheimer’s dementia, study finds

Psychopathic personality and weak impulse control pair up to predict teen property crime

Low sexual activity, body shape, and mood may combine in ways that shorten lives, new study suggests

Highly irritable teens are more likely to bully others, but anxiety mitigates this tendency

Neuroscientists identify brain pathway that prioritizes safety over other needs

Liberals and conservatives live differently — but people think the divide is even bigger than it is

Neuroscientists shed new light on how heroin disrupts prefrontal brain function

New research identifies four distinct health pathways linked to Alzheimer’s disease

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy