Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Relationships and Sexual Health

Expressing negativity improves support from romantic partners, new research finds

by Eric W. Dolan
April 15, 2025
in Relationships and Sexual Health, Social Psychology
[Adobe Stock]

[Adobe Stock]

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay informed on the latest psychology and neuroscience research—follow PsyPost on LinkedIn for daily updates and insights.

When people face stressful situations, reaching out to a partner for emotional support is a common response. But not all support is created equal—and not everyone receives it in the same way. New research published in Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin suggests that the way people express their emotions, particularly negative emotions, can influence the quality of support they receive from romantic partners. Across three studies, the researchers found that expressing more negativity—through sadness, worry, or frustration—tended to elicit higher-quality support, even when the stressor was relatively minor.

Although receiving emotional support from close partners is linked to better coping and psychological well-being, research has mostly focused on the person giving support. Less is known about how support-seekers might influence the kind of help they receive. The authors, Kirby Sigler and Amanda Forest from the University of Pittsburgh, wanted to understand whether expressing negative emotions helps people receive better emotional support.

“I was interested in this topic because social support is an important contributor to individual and relational well-being and I wanted to begin to understand how people can effectively garner support when they experience negative events. One way to do that was to examine how people are discussing these stressful events with their partner and how they may communicate,” explained study author Kirby Sigler, a graduate student at the University of Pittsburgh and member of the Pitt Relationship Processes Lab.

The researchers conducted three separate studies to explore the link between emotional expression and partner support. Each study used different methods and types of interaction to ensure the findings were robust.

Study 1 involved 386 participants in romantic relationships who were recruited online. Each person was shown a simulated text message from a partner asking for support. The message varied in two key ways: the severity of the stressor (either serious, like a home burglary, or trivial, like ruined plans due to bad weather) and the level of negative emotional expression (low versus high). After reading the message, participants wrote a response as if they were replying to their partner. These responses were then evaluated by independent coders for support quality—specifically, how caring and helpful they seemed.

Study 2 was a preregistered follow-up with 314 participants using a similar design but a different communication format. Instead of reading and writing messages, participants listened to a voicemail from their partner describing a stressful event and recorded an audio reply. Again, the severity of the stressor and level of negative expressivity were manipulated. Coders rated the quality of each response, and participants also answered questions about their impressions of the partner’s need and their own motivations for responding.

Study 3 extended the research into real conversations. The researchers brought 104 couples into a virtual lab setting and asked one partner to talk about a real-life stressor from the past year. The other partner acted as the support-provider. Both partners then reported how much negativity was expressed, how much support was needed, and how responsive or helpful the conversation was. This study allowed the team to observe how natural emotional expression and real-world stressors influenced support behaviors.

Across all three studies, expressing more negative emotion consistently led to better-quality support from romantic partners. In Studies 1 and 2, participants gave more caring and helpful responses when the support-seeking message contained higher levels of emotional distress, regardless of whether the stressor was serious or minor. The support responses were both more responsive (showing understanding and care) and more effective at helping the partner manage the problem.

In Study 3, similar patterns emerged in real-life conversations. Partners who expressed more negativity were seen as needing more support and received more effective help in return. This held true even after accounting for other types of emotional expression, like positive or neutral comments.

One of the key mechanisms driving these effects appeared to be the way negative expressivity influenced the partner’s perception of need. When a support-seeker expressed strong emotions, their partner was more likely to think they genuinely needed help. This perception of need predicted greater effort to be supportive, especially in ways that were practically useful—such as offering perspective or helping with problem-solving.

The researchers also tested whether the seriousness of the problem made a difference. They initially expected that negative expression might backfire when the issue was trivial, but this was not supported. Across all three studies, the effects of emotional expression were not moderated by how severe the stressor was. That is, expressing negative emotions about even minor issues still prompted better support from partners.

“We were surprised to find that negativity predicted high-quality support, even for relatively trivial stressors,” Sigler told PsyPost.

Interestingly, while negativity helped elicit more support overall, it was not without some downsides. In Study 1, support-providers sometimes interpreted highly negative messages as complaining, which slightly reduced their responsiveness. In Study 2, highly negative expression led some partners to feel that the support-seeker was too dependent. And in Study 3, perceiving the support-seeker as coping poorly was linked to lower responsiveness. These findings suggest that while negative expressivity helps communicate need, it can also generate judgments that slightly dampen the support response.

Nonetheless, in all three studies, the overall effect of expressing more negative emotion was positive: partners provided better, more helpful support when they thought the person was genuinely upset.

“One key takeaway is that expressing one’s negative emotions, such as sadness, about stressful events may help communicate to others that you need support (even though this might feel vulnerable!),” Sigler explained. “However, we are not suggesting that people should express negativity inauthentically or to everyone they meet, all of the time, as expressing negativity in these ways may have different effects than what we observed.”

As with all research, there are some limitations to consider. The findings focused on short-term support interactions, long-term patterns of negative expression could lead to different results. In addition, the samples were largely composed of mixed-gender, long-term couples, which may limit generalizability. It’s not clear whether similar effects would be seen in friendships, newer relationships, or professional settings.

“One caveat to be aware of is that the negativity we investigated here was in short communications — either a hypothetical text or voicemail, or in a discussion we asked people to have for seven minutes,” Sigler said. “Another is that we studied negative expressivity’s effects on support provided by a close relationship partner. There are many situational and relational features to consider with expressions of negativity, so we would not definitively say that expressing negativity will always predict receiving high-quality support. Understanding when and for whom it does so is an important direction for future work.”

The researchers noted that future work could explore how people express their needs more clearly—whether through emotions, words, or both—and whether this changes the kind of support they receive. Another area to investigate is how cultural norms or personal histories influence responses to emotional disclosure.

“In our lab we hope to continue to examine how people can garner support when they need it, and to examine the types of stressors people are talking about and how they’re talking about them,” Sigler said.

The study, “Expressing Negativity Enhances Support From Romantic Partners, Even for Trivial Stressors,” was authored by Kirby N. Sigler and Amanda L. Forest.

RELATED

Your brain’s insulation might become emergency energy during a marathon
Dark Triad

Study uncovers shared and distinct brain network signatures of narcissistic and antisocial traits

August 21, 2025

New research highlights shared and distinct brain connectivity patterns linked to narcissistic and antisocial traits. Using resting-state fMRI and graph theory, the study found altered activity across key brain networks involved in self-reflection, emotion processing, and cognitive control.

Read moreDetails
Too attractive to relate? Study suggests extreme beauty may backfire for fitness influencers
Attractiveness

Too attractive to relate? Study suggests extreme beauty may backfire for fitness influencers

August 21, 2025

A new study challenges the idea that more beauty equals more influence. Researchers found that highly attractive fitness influencers often receive less engagement than moderately attractive ones—likely because their polished appearance makes them seem less relatable to their followers.

Read moreDetails
Narcissistic grandiosity predicts greater involvement in LGBTQ activism
Moral Psychology

New psychology research finds leftist causes widely seen as more moral — even by conservatives

August 21, 2025

A pair of studies conducted in Spain provides evidence of a striking moral asymmetry in politics: both leftists and rightists feel more morally obligated to defend progressive causes, and conservatives tend to view liberals as more morally upright than vice versa.

Read moreDetails
People high in psychopathy and low in cognitive ability are the most politically active online, study finds
Attachment Styles

Securely attached individuals are more likely to engage in BDSM

August 20, 2025

Researchers found that BDSM participants tend to report lower levels of attachment anxiety and avoidance than non-participants. The study suggests that secure emotional attachment may increase the likelihood of exploring BDSM practices in real life.

Read moreDetails
People high in psychopathy and low in cognitive ability are the most politically active online, study finds
Dating

Couples who meet offline tend to have more satisfying relationships

August 20, 2025

Is online dating changing how love feels? A large cross-cultural study suggests that couples who begin their relationships offline tend to report deeper emotional bonds and greater satisfaction than those who met through apps, websites, or social media platforms.

Read moreDetails
People high in psychopathy and low in cognitive ability are the most politically active online, study finds
Political Psychology

People high in psychopathy and low in cognitive ability are the most politically active online, study finds

August 20, 2025

New research highlights a striking pattern: individuals with high psychopathic traits and lower cognitive ability tend to be the most politically active online. The study also links fear of missing out to digital engagement across eight diverse national contexts.

Read moreDetails
Scientists use Blade Runner-esque technique to reveal how avoidant attachment influences emotional processing
Social Psychology

Ancient laws and modern minds agree on what body parts matter most

August 19, 2025

For centuries, laws like “an eye for an eye” have shaped justice. A new psychology study suggests people across cultures and eras share deep intuitions about which body parts are most valuable—echoed in ancient codes and modern compensation laws.

Read moreDetails
Obesity before pregnancy linked to autism-like behavior in male offspring, study finds
Dating

New research uncovers a psychological paradox in choosiness and sex

August 19, 2025

Psychology researchers uncovered an unexpected divide in choosiness. Singles who articulate more non-negotiables in a partner report more sexual activity, while those who act pickier in rating dating profiles report less — suggesting different types of choosiness shape sex lives differently.

Read moreDetails

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Birth control pills reduce the brain’s functional individuality

Study uncovers shared and distinct brain network signatures of narcissistic and antisocial traits

Chronic exposure to microplastics impairs blood-brain barrier and damages neurons

Too attractive to relate? Study suggests extreme beauty may backfire for fitness influencers

First-time fathers show distinct brain responses to their own babies

New psychology research finds leftist causes widely seen as more moral — even by conservatives

The neuroscience of rejection: The surprising way your brain learns from being left out

Securely attached individuals are more likely to engage in BDSM

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy