Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology Political Psychology Authoritarianism

Major study points to evolved psychology behind support for strongmen

by Eric W. Dolan
June 5, 2025
in Authoritarianism, Evolutionary Psychology
[Adobe Stock]

[Adobe Stock]

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay informed on the latest psychology and neuroscience research—follow PsyPost on LinkedIn for daily updates and insights.

A new study published in Evolution and Human Behavior suggests that people around the world are more likely to favor dominant, authoritarian leaders during times of intergroup conflict. Drawing on data from 25 countries, the researchers found consistent evidence that both perceived and actual conflict are linked to increased preferences for leaders with dominant traits. These findings support the idea that humans may be equipped with a psychological system that evolved to prioritize strong leadership when faced with external threats.

The study aimed to explore whether support for dominant leaders is a universal human tendency that becomes stronger in response to conflict. Across history, powerful figures—many with authoritarian traits—have often gained popular support during wartime or periods of social unrest. Yet, research has also shown that voters usually prefer leaders who are warm and competent. This raises the question: why do dominant leaders still rise to power so frequently, even when they may not represent voters’ default preferences?

One explanation is rooted in evolutionary psychology. Human ancestors often faced dangerous intergroup conflicts, such as attacks from rival tribes. In these contexts, following a physically dominant and aggressive leader may have increased group survival. The researchers behind this study, led by Mark van Vugt (a professor at the Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam) and Lasse Laustsen (an associate professor at Aarhus University), proposed that modern humans retain this instinct.

“Both of us have held a strong and long-lived interest in understanding why citizens and followers across societies come to prefer seemingly dominant, authoritarian and strong leaders over the alternatives,” van Vugt and Laustsen told PsyPost.

“Because we are both trained in evolutionary psychology, we both worked on projects trying to answer this question based on evolutionary models of followership and leadership. A common finding across our (and others’) findings is that the more followers tend to perceive society as conflict-ridden the more they turn to dominant, strong and authoritarian leaders.

“Thus, when we met at a workshop co-organized by Christopher von Rueden (University of Richmond) and Mark in 2017, we decided to test the universality of this relationship leveraging our professional networks to collect data across all continents and across as many countries as possible.

“Importantly, if—as we argue in our article and previous work—’the intergroup conflict – dominant leader nexus’ is rooted in evolved psychological systems then we should expect humans more or less everywhere to display increased preferences for dominant leaders when assigned to conflict situations (compared to no conflict situations).”

The research team conducted a large-scale, multi-country investigation involving 5,008 participants from a diverse set of nations including the United States, China, Kenya, Russia, and Chile. Participants were recruited from student populations, convenience samples, and nationally representative groups, depending on the country. Surveys were conducted online between October 2019 and November 2020.

The researchers designed a set of four tests to examine how intergroup conflict shapes leadership preferences. Participants were randomly assigned to one of three experimental conditions: a war scenario, a peace scenario, or a neutral control condition. Those in the war group were told to imagine their country was under threat, while those in the peace group were asked to imagine a calm and friendly international situation. Participants were then shown pairs of faces, one subtly altered to appear more dominant, and asked which person they would prefer to lead their country.

In the first test, participants in the war condition were significantly more likely to choose the dominant-looking faces as leaders. Across all countries, 54% of participants in the war condition preferred the dominant face, compared to 46% in the control condition and 42% in the peace condition. In other words, perceived conflict increased support for dominant-looking leaders, while peace reduced it. This pattern was consistent in 19 of the 25 countries, suggesting a broad cross-cultural effect.

“We were both surprised to see the high consistency of our experimental results (i.e. that subjects assigned to the war condition displayed stronger preferences for dominant leaders than subjects in the control or peace conditions) across countries,” van Vugt and Laustsen told PsyPost. “Yet, at the same time we were also surprised to see that the countries where this pattern was not supported was Nigeria and Russia. We can only speculate about the reasons why these countries depart from the overall pattern.”

The second test looked at participants’ explicit preferences for leadership traits, including dominance, warmth, and competence. Participants in the war condition were more likely to say they wanted a dominant leader, and less likely to prioritize warmth. Preferences for competence, however, remained stable regardless of the scenario. This supports the idea that conflict specifically increases desire for dominance, not just any leadership trait.

The third test focused on individual differences. People who scored higher on measures of right-wing authoritarianism and social dominance orientation—psychological scales that assess how dangerous or competitive someone believes the world to be—were more likely to prefer dominant leaders. These tendencies remained significant even after controlling for age, gender, income, and education.

The fourth and final test looked at country-level factors. In countries with a history of armed conflict or high military spending, people expressed stronger average preferences for dominant leadership. For example, countries that had participated in more intergroup wars or spent more per capita on their military showed higher support for dominant traits in leaders. Although these correlations were smaller than those found in the other tests, they offered additional support for the theory.

Taken together, the results suggest that humans may have an evolved tendency to favor dominant leaders in response to perceived threat. This “followership psychology” likely developed over millennia when strong, forceful leaders were better able to protect groups from external enemies. Although such instincts may have been useful in small-scale societies, they may not serve us as well in the context of modern nation-states and complex international diplomacy.

“We see our results as important for theoretical reasons, but also for understanding ongoing conflicts around the world and rising preferences for dominant and strong leaders,” van Vugt and Laustsen explained. “Theoretically, our results add yet another piece of evidence that humans reasons about leadership and followership based on evolved psychological systems tightly linked to perceptions of conflict. This is important for understanding how humans facing real war (e.g., the ongoing war in Ukraine) or threats of future conflicts and attacks (e.g. the Chinese threat on Taiwan) reason about leadership.”

“In particular our findings are important if one wishes to de-escalate conflicts. One take on the ‘the intergroup conflict – dominant leader nexus’ is that it creates a vicious circle in which dominant leaders are preferred due to rising conflict, but these same dominant leaders are likely to further intensify conflicts through their aggressive and dominant tactics and behaviors. Consequently, understanding that breaking this nexus is going to be hard as it likely rests on evolved psychological systems and intuitions constitutes a main take-away from our article.”

While the findings offer strong support for the conflict hypothesis, there are some limitations. The majority of participants were university-educated and recruited online, which could limit generalizability to broader populations. And while the face-based tasks were designed to subtly manipulate perceptions of dominance, there is always a risk that participants guessed the study’s purpose.

“As with most social science survey experiments, readers of our article should keep in mind that results rest on self-reported preferences by participants who may not have any concrete experience with war or conflict (yet, we also want to stress that given the wide variety of sampled countries war and conflict experience is probably higher in our study than in most previous work on the topic),” the researchers noted.

“However, in another project based on a sample of Ukrainian individuals in the first months after the Russian invasion, we find Ukrainians thinking about the ongoing war display stronger preferences for dominant leaders than Ukrainians thinking about a peaceful future (see this link for a short blog-post about the study: https://www.psychologytoday.com/gb/blog/naturally-selected/202205/war-and-the-preference-strong-leader). That is, the results reported in our current article replicate patterns obtained from individuals facing actual war providing further credence to the key message that preferences for dominant leaders are tightly connected to perceptions and experiences of intergroup conflict and war.”

The researchers are now exploring several follow-up questions. “We are pursuing different directions in future projects. Some of our work ties impressions of leader dominance to various kinds of behavior and opinion statements from leaders. For instance, one project tests if undemocratic behavior affects impressions of dominance, which—based on preliminary results—seems to be the case.”

“Another project investigates if “the intergroup conflict – dominant leader nexus” is already in place among pre-school children or, if it is not, at what age conflict becomes linked to preferences for dominant leaders. Finally, other projects investigate if other types of contexts have also molded human leader preferences giving rise to preferences for other character traits in leaders when societies face other kinds of situations and scenarios.”

The authors also noted that the logistical demands of this study were immense, requiring collaboration with researchers across dozens of countries and enduring delays due to the COVID-19 pandemic. Despite the challenges, the team believes the effort was worthwhile. By uncovering a consistent global pattern in how people respond to conflict, the study sheds light on the psychological roots of political behavior and helps explain why strongman leaders continue to appeal to so many—especially in turbulent times.

The study, “Cross-cultural evidence that intergroup conflict heightens preferences for dominant leaders: A 25-country study,” was authored by Lasse Laustsen, Xiaotian Sheng, M. Ghufran Ahmad, Laith Al-Shawaf, Benjamin Banai, Irena Pavela Banai, Michael Barlev, Nicolas Bastardoz, Alexander Bor, Joey T. Cheng, Anna Chmielińska, Alexandra Cook, Kyriaki Fousiani, Zachary H. Garfield, Maliki Ghossainy, Shang E. Ha, Tingting Ji, Benedict C. Jones, Michal Kandrik, Catherine Chiugo Kanu, Douglas T. Kenrick, Tobias L. Kordsmeyer, Cristhian A. Martínez, Honorata Mazepus, Jiaqing O, Ike Ernest Onyishi, Boguslaw Pawlowski, Lars Penke, Michael Bang Petersen, Richard Ronay, Daniel Sznycer, Gonzalo Palomo-Vélez, Christopher R. von Rueden, Israel Waismel-Manor, Adi Wiezel, and Mark van Vugt.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Donald Trump’s presidency associated with significant changes in the topography of prejudice in the United States
Authoritarianism

Authoritarian beliefs predict whether voters see Trump or Clinton as psychopathic

June 4, 2025

Researchers found that voters’ authoritarian tendencies influenced how they judged the psychopathic traits of 2016 presidential candidates. Those high in authoritarianism were more likely to view Trump favorably and Clinton as psychologically disordered—and vice versa.

Read moreDetails
Authoritarianism in parents may hinder a key cognitive skill in their children
Authoritarianism

Authoritarianism in parents may hinder a key cognitive skill in their children

June 2, 2025

A new study suggests that mothers who favor social hierarchies and obedience to authority use less perspective-taking language with their children—especially when discussing people from different ethnic backgrounds. Their children also show weaker ability to understand others’ thoughts and feelings.

Read moreDetails
Sex differences in disgust sensitivity fade with age, large-scale study finds
Evolutionary Psychology

Sex differences in disgust sensitivity fade with age, large-scale study finds

May 29, 2025

A new study finds that women are more sensitive to disgust and contamination than men—but only when they’re younger. By older adulthood, those sex differences disappear, suggesting age plays a key role in how people respond to health threats.

Read moreDetails
A single Trump tweet has been connected to a rise in arrests of white Americans
Authoritarianism

New study helps explain rising Trump support among minority voters

May 29, 2025

The belief that only conservatives prefer authoritarian leaders is upended by new research showing ethnic minorities—regardless of political affiliation—are more supportive of strong leadership than White liberals. The study suggests generalized trust is a key psychological factor.

Read moreDetails
Narcissistic personality traits appear to reduce reproductive success
Evolutionary Psychology

Narcissistic personality traits appear to reduce reproductive success

May 25, 2025

Narcissism might not be evolutionarily adaptive after all. A Serbian study found that individuals high in narcissism, particularly the vulnerable form, tend to have fewer children and less interest in parenting, partly due to relationship instability and emotional insecurity.

Read moreDetails
Fathers with more dominant-looking faces are more likely to have sons
Evolutionary Psychology

Fathers with more dominant-looking faces are more likely to have sons

May 24, 2025

New research finds a connection between fathers’ facial dominance and the likelihood of having a son. While other factors like self-reported dominance and facial structure showed no effect, dominant-looking faces stood out as a potential predictor of offspring sex.

Read moreDetails
People with Dark Triad traits gain others’ trust through facial attractiveness
Dark Triad

People with Dark Triad traits gain others’ trust through facial attractiveness

May 24, 2025

New research reveals that individuals high in narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy are often seen as more trustworthy based on facial appearance alone—leading to increased trust and financial investment from others in short-term interactions, despite their antisocial tendencies.

Read moreDetails
New research challenges idea that female breasts are sexualized due to modesty norms
Attractiveness

New research challenges idea that female breasts are sexualized due to modesty norms

May 18, 2025

A new study of indigenous men in Papua, Indonesia, found that exposure to topless women during youth did not reduce sexual interest in female breasts—suggesting male attraction to breasts may stem from evolved biology rather than cultural taboos.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Young adults who experience ghosting are more likely to ghost others

Resilience may protect against psychopathic traits in people with childhood trauma

Sensory issues in autism may stem from co-occurring emotional blindness, not autism itself

Intermittent fasting boosts libido by lowering brain serotonin

Verbal abuse changes how children’s brains develop, increasing risk of anxiety and depression

Higher body roundness linked to sleep disorders, with depression playing a mediating role

New study links psychological traits to loneliness and negative coping behaviors in young adults

Self-perceived physical attractiveness linked to stronger materialistic values

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy