Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Developmental Psychology

Moral self-concept in kindergarten predicts better social skills in early school years, study finds

by Mane Kara-Yakoubian
June 12, 2025
in Developmental Psychology, Moral Psychology
[Adobe Stock]

[Adobe Stock]

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

Children’s views of themselves as moral individuals can boost their social and emotional skills — and vice versa — according to a longitudinal study published in Cognition & Emotion.

Socioemotional competencies, such as emotion regulation, empathy, and prosocial behavior, are essential for children’s successful social functioning and psychological development. These skills are shaped by early experiences and social interactions, and they become especially important during key life transitions, like entering primary school.

A related but distinct construct, moral self-concept (MSC), refers to how children view themselves in terms of moral traits and actions. Past research shows that a strong MSC is associated with increased prosocial behavior and emotion regulation. However, less is known about how MSC and socioemotional competencies influence each other over time, particularly during the transition from kindergarten to primary school.

Tina Schiele and colleagues explored this connection by investigating the development and relationship of socioemotional competencies and MSC in early childhood. Theoretical accounts differ: one view posits that MSC emerges from observing one’s own prosocial behaviors (the self-perception approach), while another suggests that MSC, formed through early socialization, leads to later socioemotional competencies (the self-consistency approach). This study was designed to test both models using a longitudinal dataset.

The researchers tracked 500 children (~5-years-old) from southern Germany across two years, beginning in the penultimate year of kindergarten and ending at the close of Grade 1. These children were drawn from over 300 kindergartens and later enrolled in more than 200 primary schools, ensuring broad representativeness. Data collection occurred at four key points, capturing both children’s self-perceptions and teacher-rated behavior. The cohort was split into two groups: one began data collection about eight months earlier than the other, allowing for staggered tracking of development.

Children’s moral self-concept was assessed using picture-based self-report measures tailored to their age. Initially, children responded to images of peers engaging in moral or antisocial behaviors and indicated how much they resembled the child in the picture. As the children aged and entered primary school, this was replaced with a more detailed Likert-style scale allowing them to express moral tendencies (like sharing or helping) on a continuum.

Teachers, meanwhile, evaluated the children’s socioemotional competencies, such as emotion regulation, empathy, and prosocial behavior, through well-validated behavioral checklists appropriate to the child’s developmental stage. Instruments evolved over time, switching from observation tools commonly used in kindergarten to rating scales suited for school settings. To contextualize children’s social environment, the researchers also gathered data on family socioeconomic status, including income, parental education, and occupational prestige.

The researchers found that moral self-concept and socioemotional competencies demonstrated considerable stability across the two-year period, even as children transitioned from kindergarten into the more structured environment of primary school. Children who rated themselves as more moral at earlier time points tended to maintain that view over time, and the same was true for teacher ratings of socioemotional competencies. These trajectories did not appear to be random or situationally bound, but rather suggested relatively consistent developmental patterns.

Interestingly, while the two constructs were only weakly linked in kindergarten, a meaningful correlation emerged after children entered school. Specifically, moral self-concept and socioemotional competencies began to align more closely in Grade 1, suggesting that children’s self-perception as “good” or “moral” became increasingly reflected in their socially competent behaviors, or vice versa.

When examining predictive relationships, the findings supported a bidirectional model. A strong moral self-concept in the final year of kindergarten predicted greater socioemotional competence in the early months of Grade 1. In turn, children who displayed stronger socioemotional skills in Grade 1 were more likely to report a heightened moral self-concept by the end of that school year. This reciprocal pattern supports the idea that early moral identity and emotional-social abilities reinforce each other across time.

Girls consistently received higher socioemotional competence ratings from teachers and reported higher MSC at certain time points, though these differences were not uniform. Children from lower socioeconomic backgrounds occasionally reported higher moral self-concepts than their higher-socioeconomic peers, though they were rated less favorably on socioemotional competencies by teachers. These effects, however, were not consistently observed across all time points and should be interpreted cautiously.

Overall, the findings underscore a developmental synergy between children’s sense of themselves as moral beings and their social-emotional skill set, especially during the sensitive school transition period.

This research faced a high proportion of missing teacher data (around 46%), which may limit the precision of the findings despite the use of robust imputation techniques.

The study, “Good child, bad child: the development of and relations between children’s socioemotional competencies and moral self-concept from kindergarten to the end of Grade 1” was authored by Tina Schiele, Anna Mues, María Valcárcel Jiménez, and Frank Niklas.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Brain boost from pecans? New study finds short-term cognitive benefits
Attachment Styles

Parental conflict may shape how mothers discipline their children

June 12, 2025

A new study shows that when mothers experience hostile conflict with their partner, they may feel less emotionally secure—an effect that predicts harsher discipline toward their children. Fathers showed no similar pattern in parenting behavior.

Read moreDetails
Key differences found between narcissistic rivalry and narcissistic admiration in romantic relationships
Attractiveness

What drives vulnerable narcissism? Clues may lie in childhood attachment

June 10, 2025

A new meta-analysis reveals that vulnerable narcissism is moderately linked to insecure attachment styles, particularly preoccupied and fearful types. The findings highlight how early emotional experiences may shape narcissistic traits and underscore the importance of supporting healthy childhood development.

Read moreDetails
New research shows 2020 U.S. vote counts were extraordinarily accurate, contradicting fraud claims
Attractiveness

The “beautiful is moral” stereotype may be an illusion shaped by how much we like someone

June 10, 2025

New research across three countries suggests that people view attractive individuals, especially women, as more moral—but this effect disappears when liking is accounted for. The findings highlight how emotional responses shape moral character judgments more than appearance alone.

Read moreDetails
Psychopathy stands out as key trait behind uncommitted sexual behavior
Developmental Psychology

New psychology research confirms the power of singing to infants

June 9, 2025

Researchers found that when caregivers sing more often to their infants, babies become noticeably happier over time. The randomized trial used real-time mood tracking and showed that even a brief music enrichment intervention can shape emotional development in infancy.

Read moreDetails
Kids start associating accents with intelligence surprisingly early
Developmental Psychology

Kids start associating accents with intelligence surprisingly early

June 6, 2025

A new study suggests that children as young as five already associate certain British accents with intelligence or lack of it. The findings highlight how early accent-based biases form—and how exposure to accent diversity at home may reduce them.

Read moreDetails
Antidepressant use in pregnancy not linked to child mental health problems after accounting for maternal depression
Depression

Childhood curiosity might be somewhat protective against depression in adulthood, study finds

June 4, 2025

Childhood curiosity may offer modest protection against adult depression, according to a large study from China. Researchers found that this link was explained, in part, by confidence in the future—especially for women.

Read moreDetails
Mindfulness boosts generosity only for group-oriented individuals
Mindfulness

Mindfulness boosts generosity only for group-oriented individuals

May 31, 2025

A new study suggests that mindfulness doesn’t make everyone more helpful. People who see themselves as connected to others tend to be more generous when mindful, while highly independent individuals may become less inclined to help.

Read moreDetails
Scientists identify distinct brain patterns linked to mental health symptoms
Moral Psychology

Your bodily awareness guides your morality, new neuroscience study suggests

May 20, 2025

Researchers found that interoceptive awareness—the ability to sense internal bodily states—predicts whether people’s moral judgments match group norms. Brain scans revealed that resting-state activity in specific brain regions mediates this relationship.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Fears about AI push workers to embrace creativity over coding, new research suggests

Flipping two atoms in LSD turned it into a powerful treatment for damaged brain circuits

Seeing struggle as growth linked to higher self-esteem and life satisfaction

New research links certain types of narcissism to anti-immigrant attitudes

Common sleep aid blocks brain inflammation and tau buildup in Alzheimer’s model

The dark psychology of how people get drawn into cults

A neuroscientist explains why it’s impossible for AI to “understand” language

Parental conflict may shape how mothers discipline their children

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy