Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Psychology of Religion

New research uncovers stereotype differences between agnostics and atheists

by Patricia Y. Sanchez
May 9, 2022
in Psychology of Religion
Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay informed on the latest psychology and neuroscience research—follow PsyPost on LinkedIn for daily updates and insights.

Agnosticism and atheism are often categorized into one “nonreligious” group in research despite these being distinct belief systems. New research published in Psychology of Religion and Spirituality finds that people do have distinct stereotypes that differentiate agnosticism and atheism from each other and from Christianity.

“I was interested in this topic because I noticed that most research on nonreligious groups either focus solely on atheists or group atheists and agnostics together. Because self-identified agnostics are as prevalent in society as atheists, I wondered whether differences would emerge if we studies these groups separately,” explained study author Veronica Bergstrom, a PhD candidate in social psychology at the University of Toronto.

Prejudice against the nonreligious can exist for many reasons. One explanation is the “moral deficiency” hypothesis, where religious people stereotypically assume nonreligious people are more likely to act immorally. Another explanation is symbolic threat, where religious people view atheism and agnosticism as a threat to the societal, American norm of religiosity.

Although research has consistently categorized atheists and agnostics into one nonreligious category, there are some deviations between these groups that could be relevant for stereotyping. For example, atheists and Christians might both be perceived as highly dogmatic as these categorizations take a stand on the existence of a God whereas agnostics do not (referred to as dogmatic recalcitrance).

In Study 1, researchers were interested in how Americans differentially stereotype agnostics, atheists, and Christians (as they are the religious majority in the U.S.). Researchers recruited a final sample of 118 U.S. adult residents from Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk), an online platform. Participants were randomly assigned to one of two conditions. In one condition, they were provided with definitions of agnostic, atheist, and Christian and asked to list up to 5 stereotypes that most people hold about each religious group. In the other condition, participants did the same but were prompted to give their own definitions of each religious group. The researchers focused on atheist vs. agnostic comparisons.

Only 56% of participants in the definition-generated condition correctly defined agnosticism. “Participants indicated that atheists are generally viewed as more evil, immoral, intolerant, pushy, rude, and satanic than agnostics. In contrast, agnostics were thought to be viewed as more confused, indecisive, questioning, cowardly, kind, curious, neutral, and scientific than atheists,” wrote the researchers. Atheists, but not agnostics, were reported to be morally worse, meaner, and colder than Christians. These patterns were demonstrated regardless of whether the participants were provided a definition of the religious groups or if they gave their own.

Study 2 sought to provide stronger support for the findings of Study 1. The researchers also included a measure of belief in God to examine whether this impacts how people stereotype the different religious groups. They recruited a final sample of 244 adult U.S. residents from MTurk. Participants were presented with and given definitions of three religious groups (agnostics, atheists, and Protestants). They were asked to rate the groups on 10 trait pairs selected from the traits given in Study 1 (i.e., immoral-moral, cowardly-brave, indecisive-decisive) for how representative they were of each religious group. Participants also completed a measure of their level of belief in God.

Results show that atheists were rated less moral, trustworthy, and safe compared to agnostics, who were rated lower on these traits compared to Protestants, supporting the moral deficiency hypothesis. Results also show that agnostics were rated more trustworthy than atheists, but less trustworthy than Protestants, providing support for the symbolic threat hypothesis. Further, Protestants were rated as older, more loyal, more patriotic, and more predictable than both atheists and agnostics. Results also show that agnostics were seen as less decisive than atheists and Protestants, but more tolerant than atheists.  Agnostics were rated as equally brave, predictable, and loyal as atheists, which contradicts the dogmatic recalcitrance hypothesis.

“Although atheists and agnostics have stereotypes in common, important differences do exist. For example, agnostics are perceived as less immoral but more indecisive than atheists. These differences in stereotypes might lead to different experiences of discrimination,” Bergstrom told PsyPost.

Belief in God was relevant in some of these ratings. For example, participants with low belief in God rated Protestants and agnostics as equally moral and brave; and participants with high belief in God rated Protestants as more moral and braver than agnostics. Similar patterns to this emerged for several trait ratings including decisiveness, loyalty, safeness, tolerance, and trustworthiness.

Altogether, findings give support for both the moral deficiency and symbolic threat hypotheses of prejudice against the nonreligious. Support for the dogmatic recalcitrance hypothesis was mixed across these two studies. “Future work should explore the degree to which context influences when agnostics are viewed as more indecisive than atheists. For example, are agnostics perceived to be indecisive about both inconsequential choices (e.g., which detergent to buy) and major life decisions (e.g., bearing children)?”

The researchers do cite some limitations to this work. “Agnostics are an extremely difficult group to study because being agnostic is not mutually exclusive with atheism or theism. In other words, it is possible to be an agnostic atheist or agnostic theist,” Bergstrom said. “Additionally, many people do not know what it means to be agnostic.”

“In our study, stereotype content did not seem to differ depending on whether the perceiver could accurately define agnosticism. However, future work will need to replicate this finding with a larger sample. Additionally, future studies will need to assess whether differences in stereotype content result in different experiences of discrimination.”

The study, “To believe or not to believe Stereotypes about agnostics“, was authored by Veronica N. Z. Bergstrom, Jason E. Plaks, and Alison L. Chasteen.

TweetSendScanShareSendPin3ShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Pleasant nighttime scents improve memory in older adults, new research shows
Psychology of Religion

Religious belief linked to lower anxiety and better sleep in Israeli Druze study

July 11, 2025

A new study of Israeli Druze adults finds that religious individuals report better sleep and less anxiety than their non-religious peers. The findings suggest that mental health—particularly anxiety—may help explain how religiosity influences sleep quality.

Read moreDetails
New psychology study sheds light on mysterious “feelings of presence” during isolation
Evolutionary Psychology

New psychology study sheds light on mysterious “feelings of presence” during isolation

July 1, 2025

New research suggests our brains may be wired to expect danger in the dark. The study found that sensory deprivation and uncertainty often trigger a disturbing feeling of presence—perhaps an ancient survival response to unseen threats in our environment.

Read moreDetails
Religion did not protect against racism’s harmful psychological impact on Asian Americans amid the COVID-19 pandemic
Mental Health

Religious attendance may not boost mental health, long-term study finds

June 19, 2025

A new study analyzing 18 years of UK survey data found that attending religious services doesn't consistently improve mental health. While previous research suggested benefits, this large-scale analysis found mostly no effect—and sometimes even slightly worse mental health symptoms.

Read moreDetails
New study identifies another key difference between religious “nones” and religious “dones”
Psychology of Religion

Millennials are abandoning organized religion. A new study provides insight into why

June 16, 2025

A new study finds that many young Americans are walking away from organized religion—not because they’ve lost belief, but because their values around authenticity, justice, and individual autonomy conflict with the teachings and politics of religious institutions.

Read moreDetails
Poor sleep may shrink brain regions vulnerable to Alzheimer’s disease, study suggests
Political Psychology

Christian nationalists tend to imagine God as benevolent, angry over sins, and engaged

June 14, 2025

Christians who believe God is benevolent, engaged, and angered by sin are more likely to support religious nationalism, according to a new study. This worldview was also associated with conspiracy mentality and xenophobic attitudes.

Read moreDetails
The dark psychology of how people get drawn into cults
Psychology of Religion

The dark psychology of how people get drawn into cults

June 12, 2025

The series Sirens plays cult life for laughs—but beneath the humor lies a disturbing truth. A psychologist explores how real-world manipulation tactics, from isolation to dependency, mirror the show's plot and reveal why leaving a cult is anything but simple.

Read moreDetails
Brain connectivity maps shed light on the synergistic effects of meditation and psilocybin
Psychedelic Drugs

Psychedelic experiences can both cause and resolve spiritual struggles, study suggests

June 12, 2025

A new study has found that people frequently described both relief from spiritual tension and the emergence of new spiritual struggles following psychedelic use.

Read moreDetails
Ayahuasca entity encounters linked to lasting religious belief changes, especially in men
Ayahuasca

Ayahuasca entity encounters linked to lasting religious belief changes, especially in men

June 8, 2025

A new study suggests that vivid encounters with perceived entities during ayahuasca experiences are associated with reduced atheism and increased spirituality, particularly among men. Before the experience, men were less religious—but gender differences disappeared afterward.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Neuroscientists shed new light on how heroin disrupts prefrontal brain function

New research identifies four distinct health pathways linked to Alzheimer’s disease

A surprising body part might provide key insights into schizophrenia risk

Religious belief linked to lower anxiety and better sleep in Israeli Druze study

A common vegetable may counteract brain changes linked to obesity

Massive psychology study reveals disturbing truths about Machiavellian leaders

Dementia: Your lifetime risk may be far greater than previously thought

Psychopathic tendencies may be associated with specific hormonal patterns

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy