Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology

Study on former citizens of East Germany sheds light on why people may choose deliberate ignorance

by Vladimir Hedrih
February 13, 2023
in Social Psychology
Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Follow PsyPost on Google News

A new study explored reasons why some citizens of the former East Germany chose not to view files that the Stasi, the notorious secret police force, kept of them when the archives were opened in 1991. Aside from claiming that the information is not relevant, most people stated that they wanted to avoid finding out that one of their colleagues or family members was a Stasi informant and that viewing those files would impact their ability to trust others. The study was published in Cognition.

Curiosity, the desire to obtain knowledge, is one of the defining traits of human beings. Yet there are situations when people willingly choose not to know. This phenomenon — deliberate ignorance — has been attracting a growing interest from researchers in various scientific disciplines.

When a society faces a fundamental transition, such as moving from war to peace or from dictatorship to democracy, people must find ways to interpret, remember or ignore past experiences and include that interpretation into the collective memory of the group in a way that allows the society to move forward.

In doing this, people can choose to ignore the misdeeds of the past in order to stabilize current power relations and ignore conflict or they can remember them in order to seek justice and prevent the defeated ideology from resurging. While the first approach can leave psychological traumas and wounds unhealed and create what is referred to as “neglected trauma” of the social group, the other approach can, in time, lead to the persecution of the innocent and create new social divisions.

“People’s individual choices of how to deal with the past unfold against the background of collective memory politics. However, because the rise of memory studies has thus far focused on the collective level, little is known about how societal transformations impact individual decisions on whether and how to remember the past,” the authors of the new study wrote.

They add that the opening of the archives of the Stasi in 1991 provides a unique opportunity to study how dealing with memories of the past functions on the individual level. The Stasi at one point had almost 300,000 employees and unofficial collaborators in a country with a population of around 16 million people. One in every 50 East German citizens worked for the Stasi. It tracked and wiretapped East Germans on a massive scale.

When archives were opened, it became a social norm for citizens to view their files. After the reunification of Germany, leading politicians called on citizens to view their files, arguing that through this action citizens could “reclaim the life that had been stolen from them—realizing, for instance, that they had been prevented from travelling, attending university, or pursuing a particular career path as part of the Stasi’s psychological warfare.” Yet, in spite of this, some citizens chose not to view their files.

The authors of this study wanted to explore the reasons for this choice. They interviewed 22 citizens of former East Germany and administered a survey to an additional 134. In the interview, participants were asked about the reasons for not viewing their Stasi files. The survey asked participants to check reasons for not wanting to view the files from a list of reasons that was provided. All participants were at least 47 years of age.

Almost 4 out of every 5 respondents listed that the information contained in the files is not relevant. However, lack of relevance was rarely the only reason listed. The second most common reason had to do with preserving social relationships and the ability to trust others. Worries that, in the files, they would discover that their colleagues or family members were Stasi informants was listed by more than half of participants. 44% stated that the findings would impact their ability to trust others.

These reasons were correlated – people who cited their worries about family or colleagues being the informants also tended to state that the information in the file might impact their ability to trust others or their feeling of being able to accurately judge the character of others.

Problems with bureaucracy, utility and credibility of information and political convictions as well as discontent with how East Germany is remembered were also listed as reasons but not as often. Interviews yielded similar results – participants most often disputed the relevance of the data, but many of them voiced worries that their close others were informants and that information confirming that could cause major discord in their families and social circles, which they wanted to avoid.

“Deliberate ignorance can be an adaptive tool for managing emotional threats and maintaining social relations, in transformative settings and beyond. Learning about others’ wrongdoings can tear families and friends apart and undermine people’s ability to trust at a moment when they may already feel extremely insecure and even more dependent on a close-knit social network. Deliberate ignorance can help to control the potentially detrimental effects of this kind of knowledge. It can also be used to preserve a positive self-image and to dodge difficult personal truths. Yet truth and knowledge are essential to redress sins of the past,” the study authors conclude.

The study contributes to scientific knowledge on deliberate ignorance. However, it also has limitations that need to be taken into account. Notably, the topic of the Stasi archives is very specific to the East German context and the study did not take into account the costs and benefits of ignoring or accepting the information contained in the archives. In situations when information is more obviously beneficial, results might be different. Finally, the study was based on self-reports of motivation and people may not be aware or willing to publicly share their ’true’ motives.

The study, “Why people choose deliberate ignorance in times of societal transformation”, was authored by Ralph Hertwig and Dagmar Ellerbrock.

TweetSendScanShareSendPin14ShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Liberals and conservatives live differently — but people think the divide is even bigger than it is
Psychopathy

Psychopathic personality and weak impulse control pair up to predict teen property crime

July 12, 2025

Psychopathic traits alone don’t always lead to serious offending—but when combined with weak self-regulation, they may significantly raise the risk. A new study reveals how these factors interact to shape property crime patterns in adolescents already involved in the justice system.

Read moreDetails
Liberals and conservatives live differently — but people think the divide is even bigger than it is
Political Psychology

Liberals and conservatives live differently — but people think the divide is even bigger than it is

July 12, 2025

New research finds that students with different political identities tend to engage in slightly different everyday behaviors. But students dramatically overestimate how much liberals and conservatives differ, fueling a distorted sense of social and political division.

Read moreDetails
A surprising body part might provide key insights into schizophrenia risk
Machiavellianism

Massive psychology study reveals disturbing truths about Machiavellian leaders

July 11, 2025

For employees working under a manipulative boss, the damage is clear: lower job satisfaction and higher burnout. A study in the Journal of Organizational Behavior confirms this toxic impact, yet reveals these leaders aren't always penalized for their destructive behavior.

Read moreDetails
Psychopathic tendencies may be associated with specific hormonal patterns
Psychopathy

Psychopathic tendencies may be associated with specific hormonal patterns

July 10, 2025

What if the roots of psychopathy could be traced in our blood? New research is looking beyond psychology and into our endocrine system. A new study suggests hormones like cortisol and testosterone may hold important clues to a person's manipulative and impulsive tendencies.

Read moreDetails
Testosterone shifts political preferences in weakly affiliated Democratic men, study finds
Political Psychology

Testosterone shifts political preferences in weakly affiliated Democratic men, study finds

July 10, 2025

What if the key to swaying a swing voter lies in their biology? New research found giving testosterone to weakly affiliated Democratic men made them less loyal to their party and more open to Republicans, revealing a potential hormonal link to political persuasion.

Read moreDetails
Bullshit is deemed more credible if attributed to a scientist, compared to a spiritual guru
Social Psychology

Scientists who relocate more often start Nobel research up to two years earlier

July 8, 2025

A new study of Nobel Prize winners suggests that scientists who change locations or work in multiple places tend to begin their groundbreaking research earlier, highlighting how exposure to diverse environments may help spark innovative, high-impact ideas.

Read moreDetails
The most popular dementia videos on TikTok tend to have the lowest quality, study find
Addiction

People with short-video addiction show altered brain responses during decision-making

July 8, 2025

People who frequently use short-video apps like TikTok may show reduced loss sensitivity and impulsive decision-making, according to a new neuroimaging study that links addictive use patterns to changes in brain activity during risky choices.

Read moreDetails
People with psychopathic traits fail to learn from painful outcomes
Narcissism

National narcissism linked to emotional impairments and dehumanization, new study finds

July 7, 2025

A new study suggests that people who see their nation as uniquely important often struggle with recognizing emotions and experience more anger and contempt—factors that may help explain why they’re more likely to dehumanize both outsiders and fellow citizens.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Frequent egg consumption linked to lower risk of Alzheimer’s dementia, study finds

Psychopathic personality and weak impulse control pair up to predict teen property crime

Low sexual activity, body shape, and mood may combine in ways that shorten lives, new study suggests

Highly irritable teens are more likely to bully others, but anxiety mitigates this tendency

Neuroscientists identify brain pathway that prioritizes safety over other needs

Liberals and conservatives live differently — but people think the divide is even bigger than it is

Neuroscientists shed new light on how heroin disrupts prefrontal brain function

New research identifies four distinct health pathways linked to Alzheimer’s disease

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy