Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

Study suggests magicians unconsciously blink their eyes to manipulate the audience during difficult tricks

by Beth Ellwood
September 1, 2022
in Cognitive Science, Social Psychology
Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Don't miss out! Follow PsyPost on Bluesky!

A new study sheds light on an unconscious tactic that magicians may be using to deceive their audiences. The experiment, published in the journal Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, found that magicians increase their eye blinking when performing difficult tricks. The researchers suggest that this tactic may be used to encourage synchronized blinking in the audience, so spectators are more likely to miss deceptive actions.

Magicians are known to manipulate the consciousness of their audiences. In 2016, a study by Wiseman and Nakano found that spectators demonstrate synchronized blinking while watching magic tricks, particularly during moments when secret actions are being carried out by the magician. This suggests that magicians may be misdirecting the audience by encouraging them to blink — and thus, to relax their attention — during moments of deception.

While this manipulation tactic seems impressive, there is some worry that a magician’s blinking behavior could lead to their own demise. Study author Anthony S. Barnhart and his coauthors note that magicians often rehearse their tricks in front of a mirror, and anecdotal evidence suggests they have a habit of blinking while practicing acts of deception.

“Before I was a scientist, I was a professional magician,” explained Barnhart, an associate professor and the Chair of Psychological Science at Carthage College. “My experiences as a magical performer showed me just how fallible human perception and memory can be, so as a psychologist I frequently turn to magicians as a source of insights about the mind that have yet to be tested. When learning magic, I was warned about the tendency for magicians to blink their eyes while carrying out sleight of hand in a rehearsal setting before a mirror, thereby blinding themselves to any evidence of their proficiency (or lack thereof) with the deceptive action.

“I decided to seek out empirical evidence of this behavior after perusing the literature on self-deception and realizing that evidence to support the existence of deep self-deception (where a person both knows the truth and actively pushes that truth out of their consciousness) was scant. If I found evidence for this blinking behavior in magicians, it would have been some of the first strong evidence of deep self-deception in the literature.”

To explore this possibility, the researchers recruited a sample of 11 magicians who had been practicing magic for anywhere between six months and 50 years. The magicians watched a tutorial for a magic coin trick involving 10 sleights of hand — a term for skillful hand movements used to deceive spectators. A week later, the magicians were filmed as they performed the magic trick four times — twice in a rehearsal setting in front of a mirror and twice in a performance setting in front of a video camera.

The researchers analyzed the footage of each performance and identified frames during which magicians were practicing sleights of hand (experimental frames) or not engaging in sleights of hand (control frames). They then coded these frames, noting whether participants’ eyes were open or closed. Finally, they performed an analysis to see whether participants’ eye blinking differed as a function of condition (rehearsal vs. performance) and frame type (experimental vs. control).

The findings revealed that the magicians increased their blinking in moments where they were practicing acts of deception (i.e., within experimental frames). However, contrary to the researchers’ hypothesis, this was only true in the performance condition, when the magicians were performing the trick for a video audience. Further evidence revealed that the magicians blinked more frequently when performing the most difficult acts of deception — suggesting that their blink rates increased with cognitive effort.

Barnhart and his colleagues say their findings suggest that the magicians’ blinking during acts of deception did not function to deceive performers about their own proficiency. They suggest instead that it served to encourage the audience to blink as well, helping the magicians conceal their secret actions.

“While my magician participants were more likely to blink their eyes when carrying out deceptive action than when not, this tendency was increased in a performance setting without a mirror compared to a rehearsal setting with a mirror,” Barnhart explained to PsyPost. “This was surprising and didn’t match the predictions from the world of magic. Our pattern of results points to a potentially interesting phenomenon: We suggest that magicians may be blinking their eyes when they carry out sleight of hand to encourage their audiences to do the same, thereby blinding the audience to any evidence of the magician’s shenanigans.”

Notably, eye blinking tends to occur during moments when visual information is scarce. If a magician increases their blink rate, this could signal to the audience that there is nothing important to see. “Although blinking during performance could act as a ‘tell’ for the audience, it could also offer a nudge to the audience that they have reached a moment when there is very little useful information in the visual stream,” the authors explained.

An important limitation of the study was that the researchers were only able to recruit a small sample of magicians due to the demanding nature of the experiment. Nevertheless, a large amount of data was collected from each participant, with researchers analyzing an average of 9,339 video frames per magician.

“Our interpretation of this surprising pattern of results is post hoc,” Barnhart noted. “Our actual experiment is agnostic to the source of this pattern. However, the pattern does conform to findings from the blink entrainment literature, which shows that media viewers are apt to entrain their blinks to those of a speaker if they are processing narrative content. Future research should explore whether a magician’s blinking behaviors impact the blinking behaviors of their audience and thereby their audience’s perceptions of the magic.”

“This work is being published in a special section of Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice edited by the leadership of the Science of Magic Association, an organization that promotes rigorous research directed toward understanding the nature, function, and underlying mechanisms of magic,” Barnhart added. “I encourage readers to follow the group’s activities at https://scienceofmagicassoc.org/.”

The study, “Tactical Blinking in Magicians: A Tool for Self- and Other-Deception”, was authored by Anthony S. Barnhart, Kaitlyn Richardson, and Shawn Eric.

TweetSendScanShareSendPin1ShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

From fireflies to brain cells: Unraveling the complex web of synchrony in networks
Addiction

Understanding “neuronal ensembles” could revolutionize addiction treatment

July 3, 2025

The same brain system that rewards you for a delicious meal is hijacked by drugs like fentanyl. A behavioral neuroscientist explains how understanding the specific memories behind these rewards is the key to treating addiction without harming our essential survival instincts.

Read moreDetails
Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time
Infidelity

Not bothered by celebrity infidelity? This psychological trait might be why

July 3, 2025

The online shaming of unfaithful celebrities is a modern spectacle, but why do some join in while others don't? Researchers exploring this puzzle found a key predictor: a belief in a just world, which unexpectedly dampens outrage and blame.

Read moreDetails
Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time
Mental Health

New research suggests the conservative mental health advantage is a myth

July 3, 2025

Do conservatives really have better mental well-being than liberals? A new study suggests the answer depends entirely on how you ask. The well-known ideological gap disappears when "mental health" is replaced with the less-stigmatized phrase "overall mood."

Read moreDetails
Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time
Memory

Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time

July 3, 2025

Our perception of time is more fragile than we think. Scientists have uncovered a powerful illusion where repeated exposure to information makes us misremember it as happening much further in the past, significantly distorting our mental timelines.

Read moreDetails
Peppermint tea boosts memory and attention—but why?
Cognitive Science

Peppermint tea boosts memory and attention—but why?

July 2, 2025

Can a cup of peppermint tea sharpen your mind? A new study suggests it can—but not in the way scientists expected. Improved memory and attention followed the tea, but increased brain blood flow wasn't the reason why.

Read moreDetails
Scientists reveal ChatGPT’s left-wing bias — and how to “jailbreak” it
Artificial Intelligence

ChatGPT and “cognitive debt”: New study suggests AI might be hurting your brain’s ability to think

July 1, 2025

Researchers at MIT investigated how writing with ChatGPT affects brain activity and recall. Their findings indicate that reliance on AI may lead to reduced mental engagement, prompting concerns about cognitive “offloading” and its implications for education.

Read moreDetails
New psychology study sheds light on mysterious “feelings of presence” during isolation
Cognitive Science

Vagus nerve signals influence food intake more in higher socio-economic groups

July 1, 2025

Researchers have found that internal physiological cues—like signals from the vagus nerve—play a stronger role in guiding eating behavior among wealthier individuals, offering new insight into why socio-economic status is linked to differences in diet and health.

Read moreDetails
New psychology study sheds light on mysterious “feelings of presence” during isolation
Political Psychology

People who think “everyone agrees with me” are more likely to support populism

July 1, 2025

People who wrongly believe that most others share their political views are more likely to support populist ideas, according to a new study. These false beliefs can erode trust in democratic institutions and fuel resentment toward political elites.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Understanding “neuronal ensembles” could revolutionize addiction treatment

Not bothered by celebrity infidelity? This psychological trait might be why

Genetic factors may influence how well exercise buffers against childhood trauma

Tips for parents in talking with your kids about your partner’s mental illness

Subjective cognitive struggles strongly linked to social recovery in depression

New research suggests the conservative mental health advantage is a myth

FACT CHECK: Does cheese cause nightmares? Here’s what the science actually says

Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy