Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

Vagus nerve signals influence food intake more in higher socio-economic groups

by Eric W. Dolan
July 1, 2025
in Cognitive Science
[Adobe Stock]

[Adobe Stock]

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Follow PsyPost on Google News

A new study published in Food Quality and Preference suggests that the way our bodies signal hunger and fullness might be shaped by our socio-economic background. Researchers found that signals from the vagus nerve—an important biological pathway connecting the gut and brain—were more strongly linked to food consumption in individuals from higher socio-economic backgrounds. Individuals from lower socio-economic groups, on the other hand, appeared less influenced by these internal cues when deciding how much to eat.

The vagus nerve plays a key role in transmitting information between the gut and brain. It helps regulate digestion, appetite, and emotional responses to stress. A common way to assess vagal activity is by measuring heart rate variability, or HRV, which reflects how flexibly the body adjusts its heart rate. Higher HRV tends to be associated with better self-regulation and emotional control, including the ability to resist impulses like overeating. Researchers have long theorized that physiological self-regulation, like that provided by vagal tone, plays an important role in eating behavior.

The current study was designed to test how vagal tone might interact with socio-economic status to shape eating habits. To do this, the researchers focused on a very specific behavior: chocolate consumption. Chocolate is a high-fat, high-sugar food known to trigger cravings and emotional eating, making it a good model for studying self-regulation. The study included 96 university students in the United Kingdom who came from a wide range of socio-economic backgrounds.

To assess participants’ socio-economic status, the researchers used two approaches. First, they asked students to report their perceived status compared to other students. Second, they recorded the highest level of education achieved by each participant’s parent or guardian. This two-pronged approach allowed them to capture both subjective and objective aspects of socio-economic background.

Once in the lab, participants were fitted with sensors to record their heart activity. The researchers collected HRV data during a rest period and also during two tasks designed to cause mild stress. One task involved mentally subtracting sevens from a large number under time pressure, while the other required participants to give a short speech about what it would be like to die from cancer.

After these tasks, participants entered a relaxation phase and then took part in a “taste test” that was, in fact, the key measure of eating behavior. They were left alone with a plate of 70 milk chocolate droplets and told they could eat as many as they liked while evaluating the taste.

The main question was whether participants’ vagal tone, as measured through HRV, would predict how much chocolate they ate—and whether this relationship would be different depending on their socio-economic status. The researchers tested three possible patterns.

First was the “blunting” hypothesis: that people from lower socio-economic backgrounds might have lower baseline HRV, leading to less effective self-regulation and more eating. Second was the “hyperreactivity” hypothesis: that stress would trigger stronger physiological responses (such as vagal withdrawal) in low-SES individuals, leading to more eating. Third was the “dissociation” hypothesis: that internal physiological signals (like vagal tone) might only guide behavior among higher-SES individuals, while lower-SES individuals would show weaker alignment between those signals and their eating.

The results did not support the first two ideas. There was no significant relationship between socio-economic status and baseline vagal tone, and vagal tone alone did not predict how much chocolate participants consumed. Similarly, there was little evidence that stress responses (vagal withdrawal) were higher in low-SES individuals or that such responses predicted more eating.

However, the third hypothesis—the dissociation hypothesis—was supported. Among participants from higher socio-economic backgrounds, higher baseline vagal tone was linked to eating fewer chocolates. This suggests that in these individuals, the body’s internal regulation systems were more closely aligned with their eating behavior. In contrast, among those from lower socio-economic backgrounds, there was no significant link between vagal tone and chocolate consumption. Even though their bodies may have sent signals of satiety or restraint, those signals did not appear to influence how much they ate.

These patterns held even after accounting for factors like hunger, body mass index, how much participants liked the chocolate, and whether they were actively trying to restrict their food intake. The researchers also repeated the analyses using slightly different methods and found consistent results.

This dissociation between physiological signals and behavior may help explain why people from lower socio-economic backgrounds are more vulnerable to overconsumption in environments filled with processed, high-calorie foods. If internal signals are less likely to influence behavior, external cues like portion size, advertising, and emotional triggers may play a larger role. This could lead to patterns of eating that are less in tune with actual energy needs, contributing over time to health problems such as obesity or diabetes.

One explanation for this dissociation may be differences in early life stress. People who grow up in unpredictable or unsafe environments—conditions more common in low-income households—may develop habits that favor immediate rewards over internal regulation. Other research has shown that early life adversity can reduce what scientists call “interoceptive sensitivity,” or the ability to detect and respond to internal bodily signals like hunger and fullness. Over time, this might impair the brain’s ability to use physiological signals to guide eating decisions.

“The findings suggest that while the vagus nerve plays a key role in transmitting signals between the gut and the brain, the extent to which these signals guide eating behaviour can vary with one’s socio-economic status,” the study’s lead author, Mario Weick of Durham University, explained. Our research indicates that internal signals may be more closely aligned with eating behaviour in individuals from higher socio-economic backgrounds. In contrast, those from lower socio-economic groups appear to show a weaker link between these physiological signals and their food consumption.”

The study has some limitations. It focused only on chocolate consumption and included a relatively young, university-based sample. Future research will need to determine whether these findings extend to other kinds of food, different types of stress, or more diverse populations. The researchers also acknowledge that their design does not prove causation—it cannot definitively say whether socio-economic status causes changes in vagal regulation or whether other variables are at play.

Co-author Professor Milica Vasiljevic emphasized that the study should not be interpreted as blaming individuals from lower socio-economic backgrounds. “Our study does not support the idea that individuals from lower socio-economic groups eat more or are inherently more impulsive,” she said. “Rather, it highlights that the internal regulation of eating – how our bodies naturally signal when to eat and when to stop – may function differently depending on our socio-economic backgrounds. This could have important implications for understanding broader health inequalities.”

Future studies might explore whether practices like mindfulness, yoga, or biofeedback can help boost vagal tone and interoceptive sensitivity, especially in disadvantaged groups. If so, such interventions could offer a promising way to support healthier eating habits from the inside out.

The study, “Socio-economic status modulates the link between vagal tone and chocolate consumption,” was authored by Mario Weick and Milica Vasiljevic.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Scientists find genetic basis for how much people enjoy music
Cognitive Science

Is humor inherited? Twin study suggests the ability to be funny may not run in the family

July 10, 2025

A first-of-its-kind study set out to discover whether being funny is something you inherit. By testing twins on their joke-making skills, researchers found that your sense of humor might have less to do with DNA than you'd think.

Read moreDetails
Even in healthy adults, high blood sugar levels are linked to impaired brain function
Memory

Neuroscientists decode how people juggle multiple items in working memory

July 8, 2025

New neuroscience research shows how the brain decides which memories deserve more attention. By tracking brain activity, scientists found that the frontal cortex helps direct limited memory resources, allowing people to remember high-priority information more precisely than less relevant details.

Read moreDetails
New study uncovers a surprising effect of cold-water immersion
Cognitive Science

New study uncovers a surprising effect of cold-water immersion

July 8, 2025

Cold-water immersion increases energy expenditure—but it may also drive people to eat more afterward. A study in Physiology & Behavior found participants consumed significantly more food following cold exposure, possibly due to internal cooling effects that continue after leaving the water.

Read moreDetails
Positive attitudes toward AI linked to problematic social media use
Cognitive Science

People with higher cognitive ability have weaker moral foundations, new study finds

July 7, 2025

A large study has found that individuals with greater cognitive ability are less likely to endorse moral values such as compassion, fairness, loyalty, and purity. The results point to a consistent negative relationship between intelligence and moral intuitions.

Read moreDetails
These common sounds can impair your learning, according to new psychology research
Cognitive Science

These common sounds can impair your learning, according to new psychology research

July 4, 2025

Your brain’s ancient defense system might be sabotaging your test scores. New research suggests our "behavioral immune system," which makes us subconsciously alert to signs of illness, can be triggered by coughs and sniffles.

Read moreDetails
From fireflies to brain cells: Unraveling the complex web of synchrony in networks
Addiction

Understanding “neuronal ensembles” could revolutionize addiction treatment

July 3, 2025

The same brain system that rewards you for a delicious meal is hijacked by drugs like fentanyl. A behavioral neuroscientist explains how understanding the specific memories behind these rewards is the key to treating addiction without harming our essential survival instincts.

Read moreDetails
Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time
Memory

Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time

July 3, 2025

Our perception of time is more fragile than we think. Scientists have uncovered a powerful illusion where repeated exposure to information makes us misremember it as happening much further in the past, significantly distorting our mental timelines.

Read moreDetails
Peppermint tea boosts memory and attention—but why?
Cognitive Science

Peppermint tea boosts memory and attention—but why?

July 2, 2025

Can a cup of peppermint tea sharpen your mind? A new study suggests it can—but not in the way scientists expected. Improved memory and attention followed the tea, but increased brain blood flow wasn't the reason why.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Is humor inherited? Twin study suggests the ability to be funny may not run in the family

Scientists discover weak Dems have highest testosterone — but there’s an intriguing twist

Can sunshine make you happier? A massive study offers a surprising answer

New study links why people use pornography to day-to-day couple behavior

Virtual reality meditation eases caregiver anxiety during pediatric hospital stays, with stronger benefits for Spanish speakers

Fascinating new advances in psychedelic science reveal how they may heal the mind

Dysfunction within the sensory processing cortex of the brain is associated with insomnia, study finds

Prenatal exposure to “forever chemicals” linked to autistic traits in children, study finds

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy