Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology

Suppressing truth and urination: Study uncovers a bizarre way to become a better liar

by Theresa Fisher, Van Winkle's
September 27, 2015
in Social Psychology
Photo credit: Giulio_Fornasar/Fotolia

Photo credit: Giulio_Fornasar/Fotolia

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Follow PsyPost on Google News

Some people a great liars, inventing stories and concealing truths without the slightest slip-up. Other, unfailingly-transparent people are incapable of fudging a single, mundane detail without getting shifty-eyed. But, there’s a trick, courtesy of recent psychology research, that terrible liars can use to fib like the sneakiest confidence man — and it requires nothing but willingness to endure physical discomfort. Ready for it? Are you sure? Okay: Hold your bladder. That’s right: the best time to lie is when you really have to pee.

In the study, which will soon be in the journal Consciousness and Cognition, psychologists found that people lie more convincingly when they need to pee. The connection between suppressing truth and urination comes down to self-control and, actually, makes a lot of sense.

Here’s why: From passing up nachos to blocking out a scarring experience, a wide array of emotional, cognitive, memory-related and motor challenges force us to practice inhibition. And, while these assorted processes have distinct neural bases, they all involve overlapping brain networks. Previous studies have demonstrated that inhibitory processes individually strain cognition. But, there’s evidence that simultaneously performing two otherwise unrelated inhibitory tasks, according to a theory called Inhibitory Spillover Effect (ISE), makes it easier to do both of them.

Deception, according to the study, is an inhibitory act because liars need to keep track of how they’ve deviated from the truth, think of a believable lie and monitor both their and other people’s behavior. Earlier studies suggest there’s a high “cognitive cost” associated with lying, as they’ve shown that people have slower response times when they’re spinning lies than when telling the truth.

“Researchers figured that people would become better bullshit artists when they had the need to urinate. They were right.”

For fairly obvious reasons, convincing yourself not to pee also requires inhibition. So, in the current study, researchers figured that people would become better bullshit artists when they had the need to urinate. They were right.

Here’s how it went down: researchers recruited 22 college students to fill out questionnaires about views on such social issues as abortion and the death penalty, knowing that they’d later be interviewed about their opinions. Study leaders also asked the students to drink water and, concealing the true purpose of the exercise, told the students they were participating in a water taste-test that was unrelated to the main study. Per instructions, students used the bathroom before tasting water, believing they’d have no other chance to go until the study ended. Researchers then randomly assigned half the students to drink 700ml of water, while the other half only had to drink 50ml. After a 45-minute waiting period, students sat for their video-taped interviews.

Researchers told some students to field interview questions truthfully, and instructed others to lie. The students broke down into four groups: liars and truth-tellers with urgent and less-pressing needs to empty their bladders.

A different group of student volunteers then observed the taped interviews, rating a number of physical and verbal behaviors that, according to previous studies, are relevant to detecting deception.

In a second, similar experiment, researchers showed the same taped interviews to another (third) group of students. Rather than assess behavior, these students only weighed in on whether they thought interviewees were lying or telling the truth.

Across both experiments, students who had to pee more urgently (because they drank more water in the fabricated taste-test) both exhibited fewer behavioral cues associated with deception and more convincingly lied about their opinions than students who’d consumed less water. Those who really had to pee also invented longer, more complex lies, which researchers interpreted as indicative of greater cognitive control.(Interestingly enough, levels of bladder fullness didn’t appear to have the same impact on truth-telling)

So, next time you call in “sick”, do it after a few too many Poland Springs. Worth a shot, anyway.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Scientists uncover potential genetic mechanisms behind the sex bias observed in autism
Political Psychology

Genetic essentialism more common among supporters of radical right-wing parties

May 21, 2025

A new study finds that people who support far-right populist parties in Sweden and Norway are more likely to believe that genes determine social traits, raising concerns about the political implications of genetic essentialism and its links to discrimination.

Read moreDetails
Narcissism negatively predicts partner-enhancement in romantic relationships
Narcissism

Narcissists are more likely to become addicted to social networking sites

May 21, 2025

A study tracking Polish adults over eight months finds that nearly all narcissism types, except sanctity, are tied to social networking site addiction. Some traits even show a bidirectional relationship with addiction symptoms, shedding light on digital-age personality dynamics.

Read moreDetails
New study highlights power—not morality—as key motivator behind competitive victimhood
Social Psychology

New study highlights power—not morality—as key motivator behind competitive victimhood

May 21, 2025

A replication study conducted during the first wave of COVID-19 confirms that need for power, rather than moral concerns, best predicts competitive victimhood in Israeli Arabs and Jews, pointing to deeply rooted motivations in intergroup conflict.

Read moreDetails
Neuroforecasting: New research shows brain activity can predict crowd preferences
Business

Neuroforecasting: New research shows brain activity can predict crowd preferences

May 21, 2025

A new study reveals that brain activity, particularly in regions linked to emotion, predicts market preferences more accurately than self-reported choices—especially when samples aren’t demographically representative. Neural signals offered consistent forecasts even when behavioral data failed.

Read moreDetails
Passive scrolling linked to increased anxiety in teens, study finds
Anxiety

Passive scrolling linked to increased anxiety in teens, study finds

May 20, 2025

New research shows that teens who spend more than two hours a day on screens—especially passively scrolling through content—are more likely to report anxiety and emotional or behavioral problems, even when accounting for age, gender, and existing vulnerabilities.

Read moreDetails
Scientists identify distinct brain patterns linked to mental health symptoms
Moral Psychology

Your bodily awareness guides your morality, new neuroscience study suggests

May 20, 2025

Researchers found that interoceptive awareness—the ability to sense internal bodily states—predicts whether people’s moral judgments match group norms. Brain scans revealed that resting-state activity in specific brain regions mediates this relationship.

Read moreDetails
New research reveals aging shifts gender stereotypes in unexpected ways
Sexism

New research reveals aging shifts gender stereotypes in unexpected ways

May 19, 2025

A new meta-analysis challenges the idea that older women face “double jeopardy” due to age and gender. While younger and middle-aged women are viewed more positively than men, perceptions of older women and men appear to converge in later life.

Read moreDetails
New study upends decades-old narrative about Democrats and the white working class
Political Psychology

New study upends decades-old narrative about Democrats and the white working class

May 17, 2025

A new analysis disrupts decades of conventional wisdom: the white working class was not a reliable Democratic base in the postwar era. Instead, support for Republicans has been a longstanding trend dating back to the 1940s.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Cognitive training may reduce negative self-perceptions in people with depression and PTSD

Genetic essentialism more common among supporters of radical right-wing parties

Enjoying nature, not just visiting it, linked to greater happiness and life satisfaction, study finds

New study finds that nostalgic memories become more bittersweet over time

Narcissists are more likely to become addicted to social networking sites

New study highlights power—not morality—as key motivator behind competitive victimhood

Attractiveness shapes beliefs about whether faces are real or AI-generated, study finds

Neuroforecasting: New research shows brain activity can predict crowd preferences

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy