Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Developmental Psychology

Two-year-olds remember things better if they sleep soon after learning them

by Vladimir Hedrih
March 10, 2024
in Developmental Psychology, Memory, Sleep
(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay informed on the latest psychology and neuroscience research—follow PsyPost on LinkedIn for daily updates and insights.

New research suggests that 24-month-olds have a superior ability to recall things they observed on a screen compared to 15-month-olds, particularly if they slept within four hours after the learning session. The duration of sleep during this period directly correlated with how well they remembered the content. The paper was published in the Journal of Sleep Research.

Research suggests that sleep plays a crucial role in memory consolidation, the process by which short-term memories are transformed into long-term memories. During sleep, particularly in the slow-wave and REM (rapid eye movement) stages, the brain reorganizes and strengthens neural connections, making memories more stable and durable. This consolidation process helps to integrate new information with existing knowledge, enhancing learning and retention. Consequently, adequate sleep is essential for optimal memory function, and sleep deprivation can impair memory consolidation and overall cognitive performance.

Scientists have proposed several theories explaining the neurobiology of this mechanism, but the superior theory remains unidentified. The effects of sleep on memory in infants, in particular, are still uncertain, with a recent study on the topic producing inconclusive or counterintuitive results.

In their new study, Neele Hermesch and her colleagues wanted to examine whether caregivers’ help and guidance during viewing of televised contents facilitates the effects of sleep on memory consolidation in 15 and 24-month-old infants. They conducted a study using the deferred imitation paradigm, a method to assess an infant’s ability to observe an action and replicate it later, as a measure of memory.

The study included 51 infants at 15 months and 54 at 24 months, with girls making up about 45% of the participants. The infants were divided into three groups: one that would nap post-learning, another that wouldn’t nap, and a control group. Infants in the nap group were required to sleep for at least 30 minutes within four hours following the learning session. The study was conducted remotely.

Prior to the study, the researchers provided participants with an actiwatch (a motion-detecting wrist device used to monitor sleep), a tablet for displaying video demonstrations, materials for the test session, and a small participation gift. Two video chat sessions with infants and their caregivers were scheduled 24 hours apart.

During the experimental session, caregivers showed their infants the study material on the provided tablet. A female model demonstrated three distinct actions on various objects in succession in these videos. Caregivers were instructed to draw the infants’ attention to the screen with comments but were told not to verbally identify the objects or actions. For other videos, they remained silent. Following these demonstrations, one group of children napped within four hours, while another did not.

Twenty-four hours later, another meeting took place where caregivers presented the previously demonstrated objects to the infants, and researchers observed the infants’ ability to accurately reproduce the actions. The control group only wore the actiwatch without viewing the demonstrations, but they were given the objects during the test session. This setup allowed the researchers to differentiate learned behaviors from spontaneous interactions with the objects.

The findings revealed that 24-month-olds exhibited a higher capability than 15-month-olds in recalling and imitating the demonstrated actions. While 15-month-olds generally struggled to imitate, 24-month-olds were able to remember the actions irrespective of having napped. Yet, those who did nap showed more accuracy than their peers who did not nap.

The presence of caregiver commentary during the video did not affect the imitation levels. Additionally, the amount of daily screen time as reported by parents did not correlate with the infants’ ability to replicate the actions. Nonetheless, the longer the 24-month-olds slept within the four-hour window post-demonstration, the better they were at imitating the target actions.

“The present study demonstrates that sleep may also help infants with the challenge of remembering information from screens. From a theoretical perspective these findings shed further light on how screen content is processed in the developing brain. Given the increasing role screen media plays in infants’ everyday life, it is necessary to increase our understanding of the complex relationships between sleep, memory, and media to provide evidence-based guidance to parents and practitioners,” the study authors concluded.

The study sheds light on the effects of sleep on memory consolidation in infants. However, it should be noted that infants watched activities that might have not been of particular interest to them. Caregiver’s guidance during the demonstrations was also very basic. Results in situations where infants remember activities more important to them and with more elaborate adult guidance might not yield equal results.

The paper, “Sleep-dependent memory consolidation of televised content in infants,” was authored by Neele Hermesch, Carolin Konrad, Rachel Barr, Jane S. Herbert, and Sabine Seehagen.

TweetSendScanShareSendPin3ShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time
Racism and Discrimination

Hispanic adolescents experience later sleep timing and greater social jet lag than peers, study finds

July 3, 2025

Hispanic adolescents reported later sleep timing, greater social jet lag, shorter weekday sleep, higher caffeine intake, and more evening screen time than peers. Screen use and stress, but not caffeine, were linked to delayed sleep and sleep misalignment.

Read moreDetails
From fireflies to brain cells: Unraveling the complex web of synchrony in networks
Addiction

Understanding “neuronal ensembles” could revolutionize addiction treatment

July 3, 2025

The same brain system that rewards you for a delicious meal is hijacked by drugs like fentanyl. A behavioral neuroscientist explains how understanding the specific memories behind these rewards is the key to treating addiction without harming our essential survival instincts.

Read moreDetails
Shared genetic factors uncovered between ADHD and cannabis addiction
Developmental Psychology

Genetic factors may influence how well exercise buffers against childhood trauma

July 3, 2025

A new study suggests exercise can reduce the psychological toll of childhood adversity, but its benefits are not universal. Researchers found that a person’s genetic makeup, specifically a variant in the BDNF gene, can influence how effectively physical activity buffers against trauma.

Read moreDetails
Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time
Dreaming

FACT CHECK: Does cheese cause nightmares? Here’s what the science actually says

July 3, 2025

The belief that cheese causes nightmares is an old tale, recently revived in news headlines. A new study investigates this claim, revealing a genuine link. The story, however, is more complex than a simple slice of cheddar before bed.

Read moreDetails
Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time
Memory

Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time

July 3, 2025

Our perception of time is more fragile than we think. Scientists have uncovered a powerful illusion where repeated exposure to information makes us misremember it as happening much further in the past, significantly distorting our mental timelines.

Read moreDetails
Authoritarianism in parents may hinder a key cognitive skill in their children
Attachment Styles

New research suggests interparental conflict can spill over into a mother’s parenting style

July 2, 2025

A new study shows that when mothers experience hostile conflict with their partner, they may feel less emotionally secure—an effect that predicts harsher discipline toward their children. Fathers showed no similar pattern in parenting behavior.

Read moreDetails
New psychology study sheds light on mysterious “feelings of presence” during isolation
COVID-19

Frequent dreams and nightmares surged worldwide during the COVID-19 pandemic

July 1, 2025

An international study of over 15,000 adults across 16 countries found that dream recall and nightmares became more common during the pandemic, with sleep duration, age, and gender all playing a role in how often people experienced them.

Read moreDetails
Researchers identify neural mechanism behind memory prioritization
Memory

Researchers identify neural mechanism behind memory prioritization

June 30, 2025

A new brain imaging study shows that when people try to remember multiple things, their brains give more precise attention to the most important item. The frontal cortex helps allocate memory resources, boosting accuracy for high-priority information.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Scientists are uncovering more and more unsettling facts about our politics

People with depression face significantly greater social and health-related challenges

Stress disrupts gut and brain barriers by reducing key microbial metabolites, study finds

New research reveals hidden biases in AI’s moral advice

7 subtle signs you are being love bombed—and how to slow things down before you get hurt

A simple breathing exercise enhances emotional control, new research suggests

Despite political tensions, belief in an impending U.S. civil war remains low

Girls are better than boys at detecting their own ADHD symptoms

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy