Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology Political Psychology

Yet another study disputes link between conservatism and negativity bias

There might be psychological differences between conservatives and liberals — but sensitivity to negative stimuli doesn't appear to be one

by Eric W. Dolan
December 31, 2024
in Political Psychology
(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Follow PsyPost on Google News

A recent study published in Political Psychology challenges a popular theory: that heightened sensitivity to negative stimuli, known as negativity bias, drives people toward socially conservative political ideologies. While prior research suggested a strong link between physiological responses to threats and conservative attitudes, the new study presents limited evidence to support that claim.

The study was motivated by influential research proposing that psychological traits, such as heightened negativity bias, shape political ideology. Conservatives, it was argued, are more sensitive to negative stimuli, which predisposes them to favor policies emphasizing stability and protection. These claims were supported by correlations between self-reported negativity bias and social conservatism, as well as physiological responses to threatening or disgusting stimuli.

However, several replication studies failed to reproduce these findings, prompting the researchers to reconsider the relationship between negativity bias and ideology. This study aimed to explore whether conscious (self-reported) and unconscious (physiological) measures of negativity bias are truly aligned, and if they collectively or independently predict social conservatism.

“In 2020, we published a study in Nature Human Behaviour showing there is no direct link between right-wing ideology and threat sensitivity measured with physiological responses to threatening (negative) images. In the new study, we explored possible links between right-wing ideology and sensitivity to threats. These followed from the failed replication and were our ‘best ideas’ at the time and we preregistered them as such,” explained study author Bert N. Bakker, an associate professor for good research practices at the University of Amsterdam and co-director of The Hot Politics Lab.

To test these hypotheses, the researchers conducted two preregistered laboratory studies—one in the United States and one in the Netherlands—and followed them with a larger, registered report-style replication study.

For the laboratory studies, researchers recruited participants from local communities near universities, resulting in samples of 71 participants in the United States and 172 in the Netherlands. Participants first completed surveys measuring demographic variables, political attitudes, and personality traits. Social conservatism was assessed using 15 policy-related items designed to capture a spectrum of socially conservative to liberal viewpoints.

To measure implicit social conservatism, Bakker and his colleagues used the Implicit Association Test (IAT). This task required participants to categorize words related to liberal or conservative ideologies alongside positive or negative valences. For instance, participants matched words like “open borders” or “traditional” with either positive or negative descriptors. Faster reaction times in associating conservative words with positive descriptors, or liberal words with negative ones, indicated an implicit preference for conservatism.

Negativity bias was assessed using an image-viewing task. Participants were shown a series of images categorized as negative (e.g., threatening or disgusting), neutral, or positive. After viewing each image, they rated their emotional arousal using a standardized scale. Simultaneously, their physiological responses were measured through electrodermal activity, which records changes in skin conductance as an indicator of arousal. This dual approach captured both conscious (self-reported) and unconscious (physiological) reactions to emotionally charged stimuli.

As expected, researchers found a positive correlation between self-reported (explicit) social conservatism and implicit conservatism. This alignment suggests that participants’ conscious political attitudes often corresponded with their unconscious biases.

However, the hypothesized relationship between negativity bias and social conservatism was not strongly supported. Physiological measures of negativity bias showed no significant correlation with either explicit or implicit social conservatism, challenging the idea that unconscious sensitivity to negative stimuli directly predicts conservative political attitudes.

Self-reported measures of negativity bias yielded slightly different results. Participants who reported higher levels of arousal in response to negative images showed a weak positive association with self-reported social conservatism. However, this relationship was inconsistent, reaching statistical significance in the United States sample but not in the Netherlands sample during the laboratory studies.

This variability suggests that the relationship may depend on contextual factors such as cultural or societal influences, or it may reflect sample-specific differences rather than a robust, generalizable pattern. The effect sizes were small, further suggesting that even if self-reported negativity bias is related to conservatism, its influence is limited.

To confirm these findings, Bakker and his colleagues conducted a larger-scale replication study with over 1,000 participants each from the United States and the Netherlands. Participants completed similar tasks online, including surveys, an IAT, and an image-viewing task. The online format increased the diversity of the sample, allowing for a more robust test of the hypotheses. The larger sample sizes also improved the statistical power to detect subtle relationships between variables.

Despite the expanded scope, the replication study provided no clear evidence that sensitivity to negative stimuli plays a role in shaping ideological differences. “These findings suggest that negativity bias likely isn’t the key to understanding why conservatives and liberals differ,” Bakker told PsyPost. “This calls for a reevaluation of the relationship between emotion, psychology, and ideology in political psychology.”

The researchers followed a practice known as preregistration, where they publicly documented their study design, hypotheses, and analysis plan before collecting data. This approach, often carried out on platforms like the Open Science Framework, promotes transparency and helps prevent selective reporting or “cherry-picking” results.

“I think the study shows that preregistration is useful: we preregistered our ideas, and most were not confirmed,” Bakker explained.

The theory that conservatives are universally more sensitive to negative information than liberals has faced increasing scrutiny as new research reveals a more nuanced picture. In a study using two laboratory games, conservatives displayed greater caution and negativity bias in a survival-themed task, while liberals exhibited these traits in a financially threatening scenario, suggesting that both groups are sensitive to negative stimuli but in different domains.

Similarly, another study found that while conservatives were more concerned about violent conflict in general, liberals expressed greater concern about economic threats, challenging the notion of a universal link between threat and ideology.

“Political Psychology research by my team and others has now shown that there is not much reason to think that there is a systematic link between ideology and threat sensitivity,” Bakker said. “I think that is important to know. Yet, I also think that political psychology might need to turn to other explanations for understanding ideological differences.”

The study, “Being of one mind: Does alignment in physiological responses and subjective experiences shape political ideology?“, was authored by Kevin Arceneaux, Bert N. Bakker, and Gijs Schumacher.

RELATED

Study finds Trump and Harris used distinct rhetoric in 2024—but shared more similarities than expected
Political Psychology

Study finds Trump and Harris used distinct rhetoric in 2024—but shared more similarities than expected

August 24, 2025

Donald Trump and Kamala Harris framed the 2024 presidential debate in starkly different terms, according to a new study—but their language also showed surprising overlap in tone, emotional content, and specificity.

Read moreDetails
Americans broadly agree on what’s “woke,” but partisan cues still shape perceptions
Political Psychology

Americans broadly agree on what’s “woke,” but partisan cues still shape perceptions

August 22, 2025

Do Americans agree on what “woke” means? A new study suggests yes—up to a point. The term tends to signal different things depending on political identity, especially around race, gender, and alignment with the Democratic Party.

Read moreDetails
Narcissistic grandiosity predicts greater involvement in LGBTQ activism
Moral Psychology

New psychology research finds leftist causes widely seen as more moral — even by conservatives

August 21, 2025

A pair of studies conducted in Spain provides evidence of a striking moral asymmetry in politics: both leftists and rightists feel more morally obligated to defend progressive causes, and conservatives tend to view liberals as more morally upright than vice versa.

Read moreDetails
People high in psychopathy and low in cognitive ability are the most politically active online, study finds
Political Psychology

People high in psychopathy and low in cognitive ability are the most politically active online, study finds

August 20, 2025

New research highlights a striking pattern: individuals with high psychopathic traits and lower cognitive ability tend to be the most politically active online. The study also links fear of missing out to digital engagement across eight diverse national contexts.

Read moreDetails
The brain is shown with a wave of sound
Neuroimaging

Early brain responses to political leaders’ faces appear unaffected by partisanship

August 15, 2025

New research suggests that while the brain quickly distinguishes politicians from strangers, it doesn’t initially register political allegiance. The findings challenge assumptions about how early partisan bias kicks in during perception and suggest that party loyalty may emerge later.

Read moreDetails
People with narcissistic tendencies report more ostracism and are more often excluded
Political Psychology

Intellectual humility is linked to less political and religious polarization across the board

August 10, 2025

A large online study indicates that intellectual humility is linked to less hostility toward political and religious opponents. The effect was seen across political parties and belief systems, and persisted even after controlling for the strength of participants’ convictions.

Read moreDetails
Antagonistic narcissism and psychopathic tendencies predict left-wing antihierarchical aggression, study finds
Political Psychology

Populism may act as a “thermometer” for democratic health

August 8, 2025

Long-term data from Britain and the Netherlands reveal that citizens’ populist beliefs rise and fall alongside changes in democratic satisfaction. The research challenges the idea that populist attitudes are static traits and highlights their potential responsiveness to political reforms.

Read moreDetails
Professors who use safe space language seen as more caring—and more authoritarian
Authoritarianism

Professors who use safe space language seen as more caring—and more authoritarian

August 7, 2025

A new study finds that safe space statements can make students feel more comfortable and open in the classroom—but also make instructors seem more liberal and authoritarian. Trigger warnings, by contrast, had no meaningful impact on students’ perceptions.

Read moreDetails

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Insecurely attached individuals are less likely to go for a compromise in relationship conflicts

Conspiracy theories can significantly influence public support for war

New psychology research identifies factors that predict sexual dream intensity

Can facial fillers and Botox-like injections improve your dating prospects or how others see you?

A simple cognitive vaccine can make you more resistant to misinformation

Psychopathic men and less selective women report more sex via Tinder

Letting loose with a swear word may actually make you stronger

Pilates may help treat female sexual dysfunction, new study indicates

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy