Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology Political Psychology

Yet another study disputes link between conservatism and negativity bias

There might be psychological differences between conservatives and liberals — but sensitivity to negative stimuli doesn't appear to be one

by Eric W. Dolan
December 31, 2024
in Political Psychology
(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Follow PsyPost on Google News

A recent study published in Political Psychology challenges a popular theory: that heightened sensitivity to negative stimuli, known as negativity bias, drives people toward socially conservative political ideologies. While prior research suggested a strong link between physiological responses to threats and conservative attitudes, the new study presents limited evidence to support that claim.

The study was motivated by influential research proposing that psychological traits, such as heightened negativity bias, shape political ideology. Conservatives, it was argued, are more sensitive to negative stimuli, which predisposes them to favor policies emphasizing stability and protection. These claims were supported by correlations between self-reported negativity bias and social conservatism, as well as physiological responses to threatening or disgusting stimuli.

However, several replication studies failed to reproduce these findings, prompting the researchers to reconsider the relationship between negativity bias and ideology. This study aimed to explore whether conscious (self-reported) and unconscious (physiological) measures of negativity bias are truly aligned, and if they collectively or independently predict social conservatism.

“In 2020, we published a study in Nature Human Behaviour showing there is no direct link between right-wing ideology and threat sensitivity measured with physiological responses to threatening (negative) images. In the new study, we explored possible links between right-wing ideology and sensitivity to threats. These followed from the failed replication and were our ‘best ideas’ at the time and we preregistered them as such,” explained study author Bert N. Bakker, an associate professor for good research practices at the University of Amsterdam and co-director of The Hot Politics Lab.

To test these hypotheses, the researchers conducted two preregistered laboratory studies—one in the United States and one in the Netherlands—and followed them with a larger, registered report-style replication study.

For the laboratory studies, researchers recruited participants from local communities near universities, resulting in samples of 71 participants in the United States and 172 in the Netherlands. Participants first completed surveys measuring demographic variables, political attitudes, and personality traits. Social conservatism was assessed using 15 policy-related items designed to capture a spectrum of socially conservative to liberal viewpoints.

To measure implicit social conservatism, Bakker and his colleagues used the Implicit Association Test (IAT). This task required participants to categorize words related to liberal or conservative ideologies alongside positive or negative valences. For instance, participants matched words like “open borders” or “traditional” with either positive or negative descriptors. Faster reaction times in associating conservative words with positive descriptors, or liberal words with negative ones, indicated an implicit preference for conservatism.

Negativity bias was assessed using an image-viewing task. Participants were shown a series of images categorized as negative (e.g., threatening or disgusting), neutral, or positive. After viewing each image, they rated their emotional arousal using a standardized scale. Simultaneously, their physiological responses were measured through electrodermal activity, which records changes in skin conductance as an indicator of arousal. This dual approach captured both conscious (self-reported) and unconscious (physiological) reactions to emotionally charged stimuli.

As expected, researchers found a positive correlation between self-reported (explicit) social conservatism and implicit conservatism. This alignment suggests that participants’ conscious political attitudes often corresponded with their unconscious biases.

However, the hypothesized relationship between negativity bias and social conservatism was not strongly supported. Physiological measures of negativity bias showed no significant correlation with either explicit or implicit social conservatism, challenging the idea that unconscious sensitivity to negative stimuli directly predicts conservative political attitudes.

Self-reported measures of negativity bias yielded slightly different results. Participants who reported higher levels of arousal in response to negative images showed a weak positive association with self-reported social conservatism. However, this relationship was inconsistent, reaching statistical significance in the United States sample but not in the Netherlands sample during the laboratory studies.

This variability suggests that the relationship may depend on contextual factors such as cultural or societal influences, or it may reflect sample-specific differences rather than a robust, generalizable pattern. The effect sizes were small, further suggesting that even if self-reported negativity bias is related to conservatism, its influence is limited.

To confirm these findings, Bakker and his colleagues conducted a larger-scale replication study with over 1,000 participants each from the United States and the Netherlands. Participants completed similar tasks online, including surveys, an IAT, and an image-viewing task. The online format increased the diversity of the sample, allowing for a more robust test of the hypotheses. The larger sample sizes also improved the statistical power to detect subtle relationships between variables.

Despite the expanded scope, the replication study provided no clear evidence that sensitivity to negative stimuli plays a role in shaping ideological differences. “These findings suggest that negativity bias likely isn’t the key to understanding why conservatives and liberals differ,” Bakker told PsyPost. “This calls for a reevaluation of the relationship between emotion, psychology, and ideology in political psychology.”

The researchers followed a practice known as preregistration, where they publicly documented their study design, hypotheses, and analysis plan before collecting data. This approach, often carried out on platforms like the Open Science Framework, promotes transparency and helps prevent selective reporting or “cherry-picking” results.

“I think the study shows that preregistration is useful: we preregistered our ideas, and most were not confirmed,” Bakker explained.

The theory that conservatives are universally more sensitive to negative information than liberals has faced increasing scrutiny as new research reveals a more nuanced picture. In a study using two laboratory games, conservatives displayed greater caution and negativity bias in a survival-themed task, while liberals exhibited these traits in a financially threatening scenario, suggesting that both groups are sensitive to negative stimuli but in different domains.

Similarly, another study found that while conservatives were more concerned about violent conflict in general, liberals expressed greater concern about economic threats, challenging the notion of a universal link between threat and ideology.

“Political Psychology research by my team and others has now shown that there is not much reason to think that there is a systematic link between ideology and threat sensitivity,” Bakker said. “I think that is important to know. Yet, I also think that political psychology might need to turn to other explanations for understanding ideological differences.”

The study, “Being of one mind: Does alignment in physiological responses and subjective experiences shape political ideology?“, was authored by Kevin Arceneaux, Bert N. Bakker, and Gijs Schumacher.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

People with psychopathic traits fail to learn from painful outcomes
Narcissism

National narcissism linked to emotional impairments and dehumanization, new study finds

July 7, 2025

A new study suggests that people who see their nation as uniquely important often struggle with recognizing emotions and experience more anger and contempt—factors that may help explain why they’re more likely to dehumanize both outsiders and fellow citizens.

Read moreDetails
Fascinating study reveals how Trump’s moral rhetoric diverges from common Republican language
Donald Trump

Viral AI-images highlight how Trump engages in “victimcould,” scholar argues

July 6, 2025

How can one of the world's most powerful men also be its biggest victim? A new paper argues it’s a political strategy based on hypothetical, not actual, harm—a concept the author calls “victimcould” used to justify present-day aggression.

Read moreDetails
New study suggests Donald Trump’s “fake news” attacks are backfiring
Political Psychology

Scientists are uncovering more and more unsettling facts about our politics

July 5, 2025

Why has politics become so personal? The answers may lie in our minds. These 13 studies from the new science of political behavior reveal the hidden psychological forces—from personality to primal fear—that are driving us further apart.

Read moreDetails
These common sounds can impair your learning, according to new psychology research
Political Psychology

Despite political tensions, belief in an impending U.S. civil war remains low

July 4, 2025

A new national survey finds that only a small fraction of Americans believe civil war is likely or necessary.

Read moreDetails
Racial and religious differences help explain why unmarried voters lean Democrat
Political Psychology

Student loan debt doesn’t deter civic engagement — it may actually drive it, new research suggests

July 3, 2025

Americans with student loan debt are more likely to vote and engage in political activities than those without debt, likely because they see government as responsible and capable of addressing their financial burden through policy change.

Read moreDetails
Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time
Mental Health

New research suggests the conservative mental health advantage is a myth

July 3, 2025

Do conservatives really have better mental well-being than liberals? A new study suggests the answer depends entirely on how you ask. The well-known ideological gap disappears when "mental health" is replaced with the less-stigmatized phrase "overall mood."

Read moreDetails
New psychology study sheds light on mysterious “feelings of presence” during isolation
Political Psychology

People who think “everyone agrees with me” are more likely to support populism

July 1, 2025

People who wrongly believe that most others share their political views are more likely to support populist ideas, according to a new study. These false beliefs can erode trust in democratic institutions and fuel resentment toward political elites.

Read moreDetails
Radical leaders inspire stronger devotion because they make followers feel significant, study finds
Political Psychology

Radical leaders inspire stronger devotion because they make followers feel significant, study finds

June 28, 2025

A new study finds that voters are more motivated by radical political leaders than moderates, because supporting bold causes makes them feel personally significant—driving greater activism, sacrifice, and long-term engagement across elections in the United States and Poland.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Neuroscientists decode how people juggle multiple items in working memory

Inside the bored brain: Unlocking the power of the default mode network

Choline imbalance in the brain linked to with cognitive symptoms in young depression patients

Scientists who relocate more often start Nobel research up to two years earlier

Sedentary time linked to faster brain aging in older adults, study finds

People with short-video addiction show altered brain responses during decision-making

New study uncovers a surprising effect of cold-water immersion

Being adopted doesn’t change how teens handle love and dating

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy