Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology Political Psychology

Threats to masculinity heighten political aggression among liberal men but not conservative men

by Eric W. Dolan
June 3, 2023
in Political Psychology
Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

Liberal men who experience threats to their masculinity tend to become more supportive of a wide range of aggressive political policies and behaviors, according to new research published in the scientific journal Sex Roles. However, this effect was not observed among conservative men who experienced threats to their masculinity.

This study was motivated by the observation that there is an increase in political aggression, characterized by the use of harsh and demeaning language in debates, the coarsening of language in political communication, and a growing tolerance for political violence. This aggressive behavior is not limited to any particular political ideology but can be observed among both right-wing and left-wing individuals.

The researchers noticed that this political aggression is often associated with male politicians and citizens. They proposed that this rise in aggression could be linked to the concept of “precarious manhood” or “fragile masculinity.” In other words, men who feel insecure about their masculinity may be more likely to support aggressive political attitudes and behaviors as a way to reaffirm their sense of manhood.

While previous studies have provided some evidence for the association between precarious manhood and political aggression, there were limitations. Most of the existing data were correlational, meaning they could not establish a causal relationship. Additionally, previous research did not explore which men are most vulnerable to masculinity threats and, consequently, more likely to engage in political aggression.

To address these gaps, the researchers conducted three experiments. They aimed to experimentally assess the impact of precarious manhood on political aggression and to identify whether this influence is stronger among political conservatives or liberals.

In Experiment 1, the researchers recruited 341 participants, both men and women, through the Prolific Academic crowdsourcing platform. Participants needed to be 18 years or older, identify as either a man or a woman, and have been born and raised in the United States.

Participants completed a personality trait questionnaire called the Bem Sex Role Inventory (BSRI), which measures masculinity and femininity. Participants were randomly assigned to either the threat or no-threat condition. In the threat condition, participants received false feedback about their BSRI scores, making them believe they scored higher on feminine traits (for men) or masculine traits (for women) than they actually did.

All participants then rated their agreement with a list of 17 political policies, categorized as aggressive or nonaggressive. Examples of aggressive political policies included support for the death penalty, presidential war powers, increasing military spending, and the use of torture.

In Experiment 2, the researchers recruited 235 men from New York University and the broader community. Participants in the threat condition were randomly assigned to paint their fingernails pink, which was considered a stereotypically feminine task. Participants in the no-threat condition, in contrast, painted circles on a sheet of paper.

All participants then read a fictional foreign-policy scenario describing a potential war between two countries. Participants rated their likelihood of war, their inclination toward aggressive actions in the scenario, and their inclination toward nonaggressive actions.

The goal of Experiment 3 was to replicate the findings of Experiments 1 and 2, specifically focusing on the effect of masculinity threat on liberal men’s support for aggressive political policies. To recruit participants, the researchers approached men who were walking in Washington Square Park near New York University. The study was initially planned to have a sample size of 200 participants, but data collection was cut short due to the COVID-19 pandemic, resulting in a final sample of 64 men.

Participants engaged in a handgrip strength task as a masculinity threat manipulation. They were given a hand dynamometer and asked to grip it as hard as they could for about 3 seconds.

In the threat condition, the unit of measurement on the dynamometer was switched to kilograms, showing a lower number for the grip pressure. The researcher showed participants a chart with overlapping distributions, indicating that their hand grip was closest to the average woman. In the no-threat condition, the unit of measurement on the dynamometer was switched to pounds, showing a higher number for the grip pressure. The researcher showed participants the chart, indicating that their hand grip resembled the average man’s.

Participants announced their scores, which were recorded by the researcher. Participants then completed a survey on an iPad, which included measures of support for aggressive and nonaggressive policies, using the same policy list as Experiment 1 (with some exclusions for consistency).

The research findings supported the hypothesis that when men feel their gender status is threatened, they tend to respond with increased political aggression. However, the researchers were surprised to find that it was liberal men, not conservative men as initially predicted, who exhibited increased political aggression after experiencing threats to their masculinity.

In Experiment 1, liberal men who received false feedback suggesting they possessed traits similar to women showed increased endorsement of aggressive political policies. In Experiment 2, liberal men who engaged in a stereotypically feminine behavior demonstrated increased support for an aggressive approach to a foreign-policy dilemma. In Experiment 3, liberal men who received false feedback indicating their physical strength was similar to that of the average woman became more supportive of both aggressive policies and foreign-policy strategies.

The researchers said that possible explanations for this unexpected finding include the limitations of the dependent measures used in the study, the higher chronic concern for masculinity among conservatives, liberal men experiencing stereotype threat in politics, and the potential for liberals to exhibit a conservative shift when exposed to system threats.

“We wish to caution that, while our masculinity threats increased political aggression only among relatively liberal men, threat failed to close the gap between liberals’ and conservatives’ overall levels of political aggression,” the researchers wrote. “Indeed, conservatives displayed consistently and drastically stronger support for aggressive policies and vignette responses. This could be due to people’s longstanding ideological affinities, as aggressive political policies tend to be more conservative than nonaggressive policies.”

“Despite these caveats, political liberals’ heightened susceptibility to messages that impugn their masculinity suggests that left-leaning men should be vigilant against attempts to manipulate their politics through such means.”

Interestingly, the researchers found that threats to masculinity had the strongest effect on liberal men when it involved intimations of physical weakness or suggestions of possessing feminine personality traits. In contrast, engaging in a feminine behavior under the experimenter’s direction had a weaker effect on threatening men’s masculinity. This suggests that messages impugning men’s physical strength or suggesting feminine traits can effectively shift liberal men’s political views towards aggression.

The researchers said that future studies on this topic should seek to include more conservative participants, as the samples for the three experiments were largely left-leaning. “Nonetheless, our failure to observe an effect of masculinity threat on conservative men’s political aggression provides good reason to believe that such future work will reveal a similar ideological asymmetry in political responses to such threat,” they wrote in their study.

“Critically, future research should examine the possible mechanisms behind such an asymmetry—including political stereotype threat, conservatives’ already elevated levels of chronic masculine concern, or ceiling effects reflecting right-leaning men’s already-strong endorsement of conservative ideological stances.”

The study, “Something to Prove? Manhood Threats Increase Political Aggression Among Liberal Men“, was authored by Sarah H. DiMuccio and Eric D. Knowles.

RELATED

New research on political animosity reveals an “ominous” trend
Donald Trump

Researchers rushed to study the aftermath of the Trump shooting. Here’s what they found

September 3, 2025
Neuroscientists just rewrote our understanding of psychedelics with a groundbreaking receptor-mapping study
Political Psychology

Study finds partisan hostility drives protest participation in the United States

September 2, 2025
Trump’s speeches stump AI: Study reveals ChatGPT’s struggle with metaphors
Donald Trump

Trump shows signs of cognitive shift after 2024 shooting, study suggests

August 31, 2025
New research unravels the troubling link between polarization and attitude moralization
Political Psychology

Shock events in 2024 presidential campaign reversed typical online behavior, new study shows

August 29, 2025
It’s not social media: What’s really fueling Trump shooting conspiracies might surprise you
Conspiracy Theories

It’s not social media: What’s really fueling Trump shooting conspiracies might surprise you

August 27, 2025
Surprising link found between aesthetic chills and political extremism
Political Psychology

Surprising link found between aesthetic chills and political extremism

August 27, 2025
Study finds Trump and Harris used distinct rhetoric in 2024—but shared more similarities than expected
Political Psychology

Study finds Trump and Harris used distinct rhetoric in 2024—but shared more similarities than expected

August 24, 2025
Americans broadly agree on what’s “woke,” but partisan cues still shape perceptions
Political Psychology

Americans broadly agree on what’s “woke,” but partisan cues still shape perceptions

August 22, 2025

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Common artificial sweeteners linked to cognitive decline in large study

Researchers rushed to study the aftermath of the Trump shooting. Here’s what they found

What teddy bears reveal about comfort and care

Individuals experiencing faster biological aging are at increased risk of dementia

Young adults with different ADHD symptom profiles face distinct cannabis-related risks

Dark Triad personalities may be easier to scam than you’d think

Study finds women tend to favor sweet actions over sweet words in romantic partners

Study finds partisan hostility drives protest participation in the United States

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy