Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

A simple cognitive tendency has surprisingly profound implications for the spread of biased information

by Eric W. Dolan
February 16, 2024
in Cognitive Science, Social Psychology
(Photo credit: OpenAI's DALL·E)

(Photo credit: OpenAI's DALL·E)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay informed on the latest psychology and neuroscience research—follow PsyPost on LinkedIn for daily updates and insights.

Have you ever considered that our brains might be more receptive to learning from people we like compared to those we dislike? A recent study conducted by researchers in cognitive neuroscience reveals just that — our ability to learn and make connections between different pieces of information is significantly influenced by our feelings towards the person presenting the information. Essentially, if the information comes from someone we like, we find it easier to remember and link together compared to when it comes from someone we dislike.

The new findings have been published in the journal Communications Psychology.

The motivation behind this study stems from a desire to understand the mechanisms that underpin our learning and memory processes, particularly in the context of social dynamics. Memory plays a crucial role in our ability to learn from new experiences and update our existing knowledge. By examining how social preferences affect memory integration—the process through which we connect information across different learning events—the researchers aimed to shed light on how our social environments can shape our understanding of the world.

To investigate this phenomenon, the research team, led by Inês Bramão, associate professor of psychology at Lund University, conducted a series of experiments. Participants were presented with a task that involved encoding and recalling associations between different objects, such as a bowl, a ball, spoon, scissors, and various other everyday items.

These objects were introduced by personas that participants were led to either like or dislike, based on a range of characteristics including political views, hobbies, and music preferences. This setup allowed the researchers to simulate real-world social dynamics in a controlled experimental environment, thereby providing a window into how our social biases might extend into our cognitive processes, particularly memory integration.

To construct these social preferences, participants were asked to create profiles for their ingroup (liked individuals) and outgroup (disliked individuals) personas, choosing from a set of predetermined attributes that covered a wide spectrum of interests and beliefs. This personalization aspect was crucial, as it ensured that the participants’ biases were genuinely reflected in the experiment, enhancing the ecological validity of the study’s findings.

The core of the study revolved around the associative inference task, a method used to assess how participants could link information across separate but related learning events. Specifically, they were asked to remember pairs of associated objects presented in different contexts, with the ultimate goal being to infer a relationship between objects that were not directly linked but shared a common associative link through an intermediate object or context.

This task was carefully designed to mimic the process of memory integration in everyday life, where we often have to make connections between different pieces of information to learn new things or update our existing knowledge.

The researchers found that participants were indeed more adept at remembering and connecting information when it was presented by personas they liked. This effect was observed across multiple measures, including the ease with which participants could encode the information, their ability to infer connections between objects not directly associated, and their memory for the information associated with liked versus disliked personas. Essentially, the study provided compelling evidence that our social preferences significantly influence our cognitive processes, particularly those related to learning and memory.

Such biases in memory integration could play a role in the formation and reinforcement of polarized beliefs within social groups. By favoring information from liked individuals, we might be more likely to integrate and accept information that aligns with our existing beliefs, potentially leading to a more divided perception of reality among different social groups.

“We are more inclined to form new connections and update knowledge from information presented by groups we favor. Such preferred groups typically provide information that aligns with our pre-existing beliefs and ideas, potentially reinforcing polarized viewpoints,” explained Mikael Johansson, a professor of psychology at Lund University.

As an example, Bramão explained: “A political party argues for raising taxes to benefit healthcare. Later, you visit a healthcare center and notice improvements have been made. If you sympathize with the party that wanted to improve healthcare through higher taxes, you’re likely to attribute the improvements to the tax increase, even though the improvements might have had a completely different cause.”

However, the research is not without its limitations. The online nature of the study, though necessary for reaching a diverse participant pool, introduces variables that could affect data quality. Additionally, the use of self-selected criteria for liking and disliking individuals, while increasing the ecological validity of the findings, may also limit the generalizability of the results to other contexts or populations. Future research could explore these dynamics further, potentially by incorporating more controlled group inductions and examining the effects of neutral versus polarizing information sources.

The study, “Ingroup sources enhance associative inference“, was authored by Marius Boeltzig, Mikael Johansson, and Inês Bramão.

TweetSendScanShareSendPin14ShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Researchers identify neural mechanism behind memory prioritization
Social Psychology

People who are more likely to die seem to care less about the future

June 30, 2025

Individuals with higher mortality risk—as judged by actuarial life insurance data—tend to care less about long-term consequences. They show more impulsivity and less future planning, consistent with evolutionary theories that link time horizon to environmental and internal health cues.

Read moreDetails
Researchers identify neural mechanism behind memory prioritization
Memory

Researchers identify neural mechanism behind memory prioritization

June 30, 2025

A new brain imaging study shows that when people try to remember multiple things, their brains give more precise attention to the most important item. The frontal cortex helps allocate memory resources, boosting accuracy for high-priority information.

Read moreDetails
Scientists show how you’re unknowingly sealing yourself in an information bubble
Cognitive Science

Scientists show how you’re unknowingly sealing yourself in an information bubble

June 29, 2025

Scientists have found that belief polarization doesn’t always come from misinformation or social media bubbles. Instead, it often begins with a simple search. Our choice of words—and the algorithm’s response—can subtly seal us inside our own informational comfort zones.

Read moreDetails
Muscle contractions release chemical signals that promote brain network development
Memory

Sleep helps stitch memories into cognitive maps, according to new neuroscience breakthrough

June 28, 2025

Scientists have discovered that forming a mental map of a new environment takes more than just recognizing individual places—it also requires sleep. The study highlights how weakly tuned neurons gradually become synchronized to encode space as a connected whole.

Read moreDetails
Radical leaders inspire stronger devotion because they make followers feel significant, study finds
Political Psychology

Radical leaders inspire stronger devotion because they make followers feel significant, study finds

June 28, 2025

A new study finds that voters are more motivated by radical political leaders than moderates, because supporting bold causes makes them feel personally significant—driving greater activism, sacrifice, and long-term engagement across elections in the United States and Poland.

Read moreDetails
Openness to sugar relationships tied to short-term mating, not life history strategy
Evolutionary Psychology

Openness to sugar relationships tied to short-term mating, not life history strategy

June 28, 2025

Sugar relationships may be more about sexual attitudes than survival strategies. A new study shows people drawn to these arrangements tend to favor short-term mating, while early-life unpredictability plays only a small role—especially for men.

Read moreDetails
New psychology research uncovers surprisingly consistent misjudgments of tattooed individuals
Social Psychology

New psychology research uncovers surprisingly consistent misjudgments of tattooed individuals

June 28, 2025

New research finds that while people often agree on what a tattoo says about someone’s personality, those judgments are usually wrong. The study reveals a consistent gap between how tattoos are perceived and the actual personalities of their wearers.

Read moreDetails
Reduced pineal gland volume observed in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder
Cognitive Science

Neuroscientists identify key gatekeeper of human consciousness

June 27, 2025

Using rare brain recordings from patients, scientists found that the thalamus helps trigger visual awareness. The study reveals that this deep brain region sends synchronized signals to the cortex, acting as a gateway for conscious perception.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

New study reveals how MDMA rewires serotonin and oxytocin systems in the brain

Ghosting and ‘breadcrumbing’: the psychological impact of our bad behaviour on dating apps

Older adults who feel criticized by loved ones are more likely to develop depression

New study exposes gap between ADHD drug use and safety research in children

People who are more likely to die seem to care less about the future

Researchers identify neural mechanism behind memory prioritization

Love addiction linked to memory and attention problems

Positive early experiences may buffer suicidal thoughts in those with trauma symptoms, new study finds

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy