Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

How artists’ unique cognitive skills enhance their drawing abilities

by Eric W. Dolan
August 28, 2024
in Cognitive Science
(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

Artists often captivate us with their ability to translate the world around them into stunningly realistic drawings. But what cognitive processes enable such skill? A recent study sheds light on this question, revealing that art students possess a unique flexibility in how they process visual information. They can switch between focusing on small details and grasping the overall structure of an object, but interestingly, they tend to start with a global perspective when beginning a drawing. These findings, published in the journal Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, suggest that artists have a perceptual edge that may explain their superior drawing abilities.

Creating a realistic drawing is more than just a creative endeavor; it’s a complex cognitive task that requires the artist to break down what they see into manageable parts while also maintaining an awareness of the overall structure. Previous research has indicated that artists might excel at this balance between local and global processing, but the specifics of how they manage this balance—particularly in comparison to non-artists—were not well understood. Additionally, some research suggested a possible overlap between the skills artists use and the cognitive traits associated with autism, where individuals often exhibit a strong focus on details at the expense of the broader picture.

Given these intriguing possibilities, the researchers sought to investigate several key questions: Do artists naturally gravitate toward focusing on details or the overall structure when they start a drawing? How do their skills in processing local versus global information compare to non-artists? And is there a relationship between these processing abilities and their drawing accuracy?

To explore these questions, the researchers recruited two groups of participants: 40 first-year art students from the Pratt Institute, a selective art and design school, and 41 non-art students from a large public university. The art students were selected based on their artistic skills and represented various design disciplines, while the non-art students had no formal art training.

Participants completed a series of tasks designed to measure their ability to process visual information both locally and globally. For local processing, tasks included the Mental Rotation Task, where participants had to determine whether pairs of objects were the same or different when rotated, and the Embedded Figures Test, which required finding a simple shape hidden within a complex pattern. Global processing was assessed using tasks like the Out-of-Focus Pictures Task, where participants had to identify blurry images, and the Coherent Form Task, which tested their ability to integrate scattered visual information into a recognizable shape.

In addition to these tasks, participants were asked to complete drawing tasks, including a still-life and a face drawing, which were rated for accuracy by non-artist judges. They also participated in a copying task, where they had to reproduce complex line drawings while researchers recorded the sequence of their marks to determine whether they started by focusing on the overall form (a global approach) or by detailing specific parts (a local approach).

The researchers found that art students outperformed non-art students on both local and global processing tasks, indicating a general cognitive advantage in visual information processing. Specifically, art students excelled at tasks that required them to break down complex visual information into its components (local processing) and those that required them to integrate fragmented information into a whole (global processing).

Interestingly, when it came to the copying task, both groups showed a preference for starting with global features, but this tendency was stronger among the art students. On the more complex Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure task, which required participants to copy a detailed figure, art students were more likely than non-art students to begin by outlining the overall structure before filling in the details.

This preference for starting globally was also linked to higher accuracy in the drawing tasks. In other words, those who initially focused on the big picture tended to produce more accurate drawings, suggesting that global processing plays a critical role in realistic drawing.

The study provides new insights into the cognitive processes underlying artistic expertise, but it does have some limitations. The tasks used to assess local and global processing, though carefully chosen, may not capture all aspects of these complex cognitive abilities. Additionally, the study’s findings are based on first-year art students, so it is unclear whether the differences observed are a result of prior training or innate abilities. Future research could explore how these processing abilities develop over time and whether they can be trained or enhanced through education.

Another interesting finding was that art students did not report more autistic-related traits than non-art students, despite the hypothesis that attention to detail—a trait often associated with autism—might be more pronounced among artists. This suggests that while both artists and individuals with autism may excel in local processing, the underlying cognitive mechanisms might differ.

In future studies, it would be interesting to examine how artists and individuals with autism compare when performing tasks that require a balance between local and global processing, such as drawing from observation. Researchers could also investigate whether the global processing advantage observed in artists extends to other creative tasks or fields, such as design and architecture, and how these skills might be applied in STEM (science, technology, engineering, and mathematics) disciplines.

The study, “Artists Have Superior Local and Global Processing Abilities but Show a Preference for Initially Drawing Globally,” was authored by Jennifer E. Drake, Ariana Riccio, Rebecca Chamberlain, and Aaron Kozbelt.

TweetSendScanShareSendPin3ShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

From fireflies to brain cells: Unraveling the complex web of synchrony in networks
Addiction

Understanding “neuronal ensembles” could revolutionize addiction treatment

July 3, 2025

The same brain system that rewards you for a delicious meal is hijacked by drugs like fentanyl. A behavioral neuroscientist explains how understanding the specific memories behind these rewards is the key to treating addiction without harming our essential survival instincts.

Read moreDetails
Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time
Memory

Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time

July 3, 2025

Our perception of time is more fragile than we think. Scientists have uncovered a powerful illusion where repeated exposure to information makes us misremember it as happening much further in the past, significantly distorting our mental timelines.

Read moreDetails
Peppermint tea boosts memory and attention—but why?
Cognitive Science

Peppermint tea boosts memory and attention—but why?

July 2, 2025

Can a cup of peppermint tea sharpen your mind? A new study suggests it can—but not in the way scientists expected. Improved memory and attention followed the tea, but increased brain blood flow wasn't the reason why.

Read moreDetails
Scientists reveal ChatGPT’s left-wing bias — and how to “jailbreak” it
Artificial Intelligence

ChatGPT and “cognitive debt”: New study suggests AI might be hurting your brain’s ability to think

July 1, 2025

Researchers at MIT investigated how writing with ChatGPT affects brain activity and recall. Their findings indicate that reliance on AI may lead to reduced mental engagement, prompting concerns about cognitive “offloading” and its implications for education.

Read moreDetails
New psychology study sheds light on mysterious “feelings of presence” during isolation
Cognitive Science

Vagus nerve signals influence food intake more in higher socio-economic groups

July 1, 2025

Researchers have found that internal physiological cues—like signals from the vagus nerve—play a stronger role in guiding eating behavior among wealthier individuals, offering new insight into why socio-economic status is linked to differences in diet and health.

Read moreDetails
Researchers identify neural mechanism behind memory prioritization
Memory

Researchers identify neural mechanism behind memory prioritization

June 30, 2025

A new brain imaging study shows that when people try to remember multiple things, their brains give more precise attention to the most important item. The frontal cortex helps allocate memory resources, boosting accuracy for high-priority information.

Read moreDetails
Scientists show how you’re unknowingly sealing yourself in an information bubble
Cognitive Science

Scientists show how you’re unknowingly sealing yourself in an information bubble

June 29, 2025

Scientists have found that belief polarization doesn’t always come from misinformation or social media bubbles. Instead, it often begins with a simple search. Our choice of words—and the algorithm’s response—can subtly seal us inside our own informational comfort zones.

Read moreDetails
Muscle contractions release chemical signals that promote brain network development
Memory

Sleep helps stitch memories into cognitive maps, according to new neuroscience breakthrough

June 28, 2025

Scientists have discovered that forming a mental map of a new environment takes more than just recognizing individual places—it also requires sleep. The study highlights how weakly tuned neurons gradually become synchronized to encode space as a connected whole.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Understanding “neuronal ensembles” could revolutionize addiction treatment

Not bothered by celebrity infidelity? This psychological trait might be why

Genetic factors may influence how well exercise buffers against childhood trauma

Tips for parents in talking with your kids about your partner’s mental illness

Subjective cognitive struggles strongly linked to social recovery in depression

New research suggests the conservative mental health advantage is a myth

FACT CHECK: Does cheese cause nightmares? Here’s what the science actually says

Scientists just uncovered a surprising illusion in how we remember time

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy