Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

Largest meta-analysis to date suggests the effects of stereotype threat ‘range from negligible to small’

by Eric W. Dolan
November 6, 2019
in Cognitive Science
(Photo credit: U.S. Pacific Fleet)

(Photo credit: U.S. Pacific Fleet)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

New research published in the Journal of Applied Psychology casts doubt on the idea that awareness of negative stereotypes substantially and systematically impair the performance of certain groups.

“I was originally interested in stereotype threat because of its potential effects on test-takers. My advisor and I decided to start this meta-analysis after two individual studies on the topic were retracted due to data fabrication,” said study author Oren Shewach, a research scientist at the Human Resources Research Organization.

“While these retracted studies showed very large effects, we found that they had minimal impact on the overall threat effect. As I became more familiar with this research, I realized that some of the experimental conditions used were dissimilar from high stakes testing settings. This shaped the current focus of the paper, which is on stereotype threat in high stakes testing settings.”

The researchers conducted a meta-analysis of 212 experimental studies on stereotype threat, which included a total of more than 10,000 adult participants — making it the largest meta-analysis of stereotype threat to date.

“Stereotype threat is the situation where someone is concerned about being treated negatively on the basis of a stereotype about their group. Stereotype threat has long been thought to influence people on tests, including high-stakes tests (i.e., college admissions and employment tests),” Shewach explained.

Some studies used blatant invocations of stereotype threat, such as by telling female participants that women usually perform worse than men on the test they’re about to take. Other studies introduced stereotype threat in more subtle ways.

Unsurprisingly, the researchers found that blatant manipulations of stereotype threat tended to produce stronger effects than subtle manipulations. But such blatant statements are rarely encountered before real-world tests.

“When we restrict stereotype threat research to studies showing similarity to high stakes testing-like conditions, the stereotype threat effect decreases substantially. Overall, our results indicated that the size of the threat effect experienced in adults on high stakes cognitive ability tests may range from negligible to small,” Shewach told PsyPost.

“This conclusion is supported by evidence from: (a) studies with lab conditions similar to high stakes testing settings, (b) a small sample of actual high stakes tests, (c) studies that incentivize participants, financially or otherwise, and (d) by tests of publication bias.”

The first study on stereotype threat was published in 1995. Since then, the phenomenon has frequently been used to explain why some groups tend to score higher on cognitive tests than others.

The new findings cast doubt on that explanation. However, the results do not mean that stereotype threat does not exist at all.

“We do not question the existence of the stereotype threat phenomenon; we just question the evidence that it affects cognitive ability tests in high stakes settings in a systematic way. Also, individual test-takers may still be affected by stereotype threat. However, we are looking at systematic effects, and average effects across groups,” Shewach explained.

“Fewer than 10% of studies in our sample contained a motivational incentive that might serve to increase test-taker motivation beyond a minimal level. It would still be useful to conduct threat research in a large-scale sample with motivational incentives, also incorporating other features similar to high stakes testing settings.”

The study, “Stereotype Threat Effects in Settings With Features Likely Versus Unlikely in Operational Test Settings: A Meta-Analysis“, was authored by Oren R. Shewach, Paul R. Sackett, and Sander Quint.

TweetSendScanShareSendPin1ShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Sleep deprivation reduces attention and cognitive processing capacity
Cognitive Science

Sleep deprivation reduces attention and cognitive processing capacity

May 31, 2025

A new study shows that 36 hours without sleep impairs table tennis players’ reaction times, attention, and brain connectivity. The findings reveal how acute sleep deprivation disrupts spatial cognitive processing, with potential consequences for athletic performance and decision-making under pressure.

Read moreDetails
Neuroscientists pinpoint part of the brain that deciphers memory from new experience
Memory

Neuroscientists find individual differences in memory response to amygdala stimulation

May 31, 2025

Stimulating the brain’s amygdala during memory formation can boost recall after 24 hours, a new study finds. But the effect varies: some people’s memory improves, others’ worsens—and baseline memory performance appears to be the best predictor of outcome.

Read moreDetails
MDMA therapy: Side effects appear mild, but there are problems with the evidence
Cognitive Science

Consciousness remains a mystery after major theory showdown

May 30, 2025

A groundbreaking collaboration has tested two of the most influential theories of consciousness—global neuronal workspace and integrated information theory. While neither came out on top, the project marks a major shift in how scientists approach one of the mind’s biggest mysteries.

Read moreDetails
A common calorie-free sweetener alters brain activity and appetite control, new research suggests
Cognitive Science

A common calorie-free sweetener alters brain activity and appetite control, new research suggests

May 30, 2025

A recent brain imaging study finds that sucralose, unlike sugar, increases activity in the hypothalamus and boosts hunger, suggesting that calorie-free sweetness may confuse the brain’s appetite control system.

Read moreDetails
Delusion-like cognitive biases predict conspiracy theory belief
Cognitive Science

Delusion-like cognitive biases predict conspiracy theory belief

May 28, 2025

People prone to conspiracy theories may share cognitive tendencies with those who experience delusional thinking. Two new studies suggest that biases like anomalous perception and impulsive reasoning help explain why some are more likely to embrace conspiratorial beliefs.

Read moreDetails
Psychology study sheds light on why some moments seem to fly by
Memory

Psychology study sheds light on why some moments seem to fly by

May 24, 2025

A new study suggests life feels like it speeds up during periods of personal growth and satisfaction. Rather than routine making time seem short, researchers found that fulfilled, nostalgic memories are more likely to make the past feel like a blur.

Read moreDetails
A colorful brain on a black background
Cognitive Science

Neuroscientists discover how “aha” moments rewire the brain to enhance memory

May 22, 2025

A study using brain scans shows that flashes of insight reorganize neural patterns in the visual cortex and engage memory and emotion regions, helping embed the solution more deeply in long-term memory.

Read moreDetails
Scientists observe lasting cognitive deficits in long COVID patients
Cognitive Science

Therapeutic video game shows promise for post-COVID cognitive recovery

May 20, 2025

A new study finds that a therapeutic video game, AKL-T01, improved task-switching and processing speed in people with post-COVID cognitive deficits. While sustained attention did not improve, participants reported better quality of life and reduced fatigue after six weeks of gameplay.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Testosterone heightens men’s sensitivity to social feedback and reshapes self-esteem

Estrogen curbs fentanyl intake by suppressing brain’s reward response

New psychology research: Feeling politically excluded heightens antisocial tendencies

Here’s what the data says about who actually benefits from DEI

Adults with ADHD face long-term social and economic challenges, study finds — even with medication

Sleep deprivation reduces attention and cognitive processing capacity

Neuroscientists find individual differences in memory response to amygdala stimulation

Mindfulness boosts generosity only for group-oriented individuals

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy