New research published in Nature Communications sheds light on what happens in the brain during moments of sudden insight — and why those breakthroughs are often so unforgettable. The study shows that the “aha!” experience of solving a challenging visual problem triggers a distinct reorganization of brain activity, involving changes in the visual cortex and increased coordination with memory-related regions like the hippocampus and emotion-processing areas such as the amygdala. These rapid shifts not only help people solve problems more efficiently, but also make the solution more likely to be remembered days later.
Creativity and memory are closely linked. People often need prior knowledge to solve problems creatively, and in turn, creative solutions are typically remembered more vividly. One powerful form of creative problem-solving is insight — the kind of sudden realization that feels effortless but results in a novel understanding or solution. Past research has shown that insight is more likely to lead to lasting memory than standard problem-solving methods. However, the brain mechanisms behind this effect remained unclear.
“I’ve always found those sudden moments of insight—those ‘Aha!’ experiences—deeply fascinating. They’re powerful emotionally, giving you a rush of pleasure, and they’re also striking cognitively, because once the answer clicks into place, it suddenly feels obvious and perfectly fitting with what you already know,” said study author Maxi Becker, a postdoctoral researcher at
Duke University and member of the Mnemology Lab.
“Since my PhD, I’ve been curious about how the brain produces these moments—what’s going on under the hood, so to speak. Only recently have we been able to use tools, like representational similarity analysis, that allow us to really investigate this. With these methods, we can now actually observe how brain activity shifts as a person moves from confusion to clarity—from searching for meaning to that sudden moment when everything makes sense.”
In particular, the researchers wanted to investigate whether this type of insight reorganizes mental representations in the brain — a process they called representational change — and how that process interacts with regions linked to emotion and memory to promote lasting learning. They also sought to determine whether these brain processes work together as a network to facilitate insight and improve memory.
To study the brain basis of insight, the researchers recruited 31 participants who completed a visual problem-solving task while undergoing functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). The task used “Mooney images” — high-contrast black-and-white pictures of real-world objects that are difficult to recognize at first glance. With enough time, some people suddenly “see” the hidden object, producing a classic insight experience.
Participants viewed these images in the scanner and were asked to press a button when they recognized the object. Afterward, they rated the experience on three dimensions commonly associated with insight: how sudden it was, how certain they felt, and how emotionally rewarding the solution felt. The three ratings were combined into a single measure of insight intensity.
Five days later, participants completed a memory test at home. They were shown the same Mooney images along with new ones and were asked to identify whether they had seen each image before, whether they had solved it, and to recall the name of the hidden object. Only trials where participants had correctly identified and remembered the object were counted as successful recall.
The results showed that higher-rated insight experiences were strongly associated with better memory performance. Participants were more likely to recall the solution to a visual puzzle if it had been accompanied by a strong insight experience during the original task. These “Aha!” moments appeared to help consolidate the information, making it easier to retrieve later.
Brain scans revealed why. First, the researchers observed significant representational change in two visual regions of the brain: the posterior fusiform gyrus and the inferior lateral occipital cortex. These areas showed altered patterns of activity before and after the solution was found — suggesting that the brain had reorganized its representation of the visual information once the object was recognized. This change was larger for trials where participants experienced stronger insight.
In addition, the brain’s emotion and memory centers — the amygdala and the anterior hippocampus — showed increased activity during high-insight trials. The amygdala is known to process emotional salience, while the hippocampus plays a key role in novelty detection and memory formation. The researchers found that insight-related activation in the anterior hippocampus was particularly important for later memory, especially when participants rated their insights as sudden.
“I was actually quite surprised by how consistent the results were,” Becker told PsyPost. “Neuroimaging is a pretty noisy method, especially when you’re studying something as complex and high-level as insight. So I didn’t expect the patterns to be so clear and reliable—but they were.”
Importantly, these brain areas worked together. Functional connectivity analysis showed that during high-insight trials, the visual regions, amygdala, and hippocampus became more tightly interconnected — forming what the authors described as a “solution network.” This network was more integrated and efficient during insightful moments, indicating that the brain was combining visual reorganization with emotional and memory evaluation in a coordinated way.
The researchers also found that the strength of this network integration predicted how likely participants were to remember the solution five days later. In other words, the more effectively these regions communicated during the “Aha!” moment, the more likely the solution was to be retained in memory.
“Our findings show that when people experience an ‘Aha!’ moment—solving a problem in a sudden flash of insight—it’s not just a psychological experience,” Becker explained. “We can actually see this shift happening in the brain. Specifically, parts of the brain that store the solution relevant information (in our case visual information), along with regions involved in emotion and memory, work together during these moments. And the stronger this brain shift is, the more likely people are to remember the solution later. So in short, insight doesn’t just feel good—it also helps us remember what we’ve learned.”
But as with all research, there are caveats to consider.
“One important limitation is that we used Mooney images—these are black-and-white pictures that test object recognition—to study insight,” Becker noted. “While our findings make a lot of sense in this context, it’s still an open question whether they will hold up for more complex types of problems. Future research will need to explore that.”
Looking ahead, the researchers hope to build on this work by tracking the moment-to-moment unfolding of insight in the brain. “Now that we have a solid method to study how the brain shifts from a state of confusion to one of sudden understanding,” said Becker, “our next step is to map out the exact timing of this process. We want to see how insight unfolds in the brain moment by moment, and identify which brain areas are working together to make that ‘Aha!’ moment happen.”
The study, “Insight predicts subsequent memory via cortical representational change and hippocampal activity,” was authored by Maxi Becker, Tobias Sommer, and Roberto Cabeza.