Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

Machine learning study identifies facial features that are central to first impressions

by Beth Ellwood
October 21, 2021
in Cognitive Science, Social Psychology
Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

A study published in Social Psychological and Personality Science presents evidence that people make judgments about strangers’ personalities based on how closely their resting faces resemble emotional expressions. It was found that among seven classes of facial characteristics, resemblance to emotional expressions was the strongest predictor of impressions of both trustworthiness and dominance.

It has long been demonstrated that people form rapid impressions of others based on their physical appearances. Such quick judgments can have strong repercussions — for example, when juries are forming impressions of the accused during criminal trials or when hiring managers are screening potential candidates.

“One thing I find fascinating about first impressions is how quickly and intuitively they come to mind. For example, I might see a stranger on the train and immediately get the feeling that they cannot be trusted. I want to understand where these intuitions come from. What is it about a person’s appearance that makes them appear untrustworthy, intelligent, or dominant to us?” said study author Bastian Jaeger, an assistant professor at the Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam.

While many studies have identified specific facial characteristics that are associated with personality impressions, Jaeger and his colleague Alex L. Jones note that this type of research comes with its challenges. Since many facial features are correlated, it is tricky to identify the unique effects of a given characteristic. For example, if a face is manipulated to look more like it is smiling, these adjustments will also influence the “babyfacedness” of the face. For this reason, Jaeger and Jones set out to examine the relative predictive value of a given facial characteristic for personality impressions, by examining a wide range of facial features at once.

The researchers analyzed a dataset from the Chicago Face Database, which included 597 faces of individuals maintaining a neutral expression in front of a plain background. The dataset had previously been presented to a sample of 1,087 raters who each rated a subset of 10 faces on a wide range of characteristics. These characteristics included attractiveness, unusualness, babyfacedness, dominance, and trustworthiness of the face. The sample also rated the extent that faces resembled six emotional expressions — happiness, sadness, anger, disgust, fear, and surprise.

In total, the database included information on 28 facial features which the researchers divided into seven categories: demographics, morphological features, facial width-to-height ratio (fWHR), perceived attractiveness, perceived unusualness, perceived babyfacedness, and emotion resemblance.

Using machine learning, Jaeger and Jones tested the predictive value of each of these classes of facial features for impressions of trustworthiness and dominance. It was found that resemblance to emotional expressions was the best predictor for perceptions of both trustworthiness and dominance. Emotion resemblance also explained the most variance in perceptions of trustworthiness and dominance out of all seven classes.

Next, using regression analysis, the researchers examined the relative predictive value of each of the 28 facial features. Here, they found that resemblance to a happy expression was the strongest predictor of trustworthiness. Attractiveness and being Asian were also substantial positive predictors, and resemblance to an angry expression was a fairly strong negative predictor. For perceptions of dominance, resemblance to an angry expression was the strongest positive predictor, and being female was the strongest negative predictor. Contrary to previous findings, fWHR was not a strong predictor of either trustworthiness or dominance perceptions.

The study’s authors say this pattern of findings is in line with a phenomenon called emotion overgeneralization, which posits that people are especially sensitive to reading emotions in other people’s faces since emotions convey highly relevant social information. Because of this oversensitivity, people end up detecting emotions even in neutral faces that “structurally resemble emotional expressions.” This information is then used to infer personality characteristics from the face, such as trustworthiness.

“We shouldn’t be too confident in our first impressions,” Jaeger told PsyPost. “They might come to mind easily and effortlessly, but not because we are so good at judging others. Rather, it seems like our oversensitive emotion detection system makes us ‘see things’ in others’ faces. Even when a person is not sending any emotional signals, we might detect a smile, just because the corners of their mouth are slightly tilted upwards. And because of our tendency to overgeneralize from emotional states to psychological traits, we not only think that they are happy right now, but that they are happy, outgoing, and trustworthy in general.”

Notably, the results imply that there are additional features that relate to impression formation that the study did not test for. “Emotion resemblances explained 53% and 42% of the variance in trustworthiness and dominance perceptions,” Jaeger and Jones report. “Even the optimized Elastic Net models explained around 68% of the variance, indicating there are other important factors contributing to personality impressions.” Future studies should attempt to uncover more predictors and shed additional light on the relative importance of specific facial features.

“Our findings are based on relatively large and demographically diverse samples of raters and targets, but they were all from the United States,” Jaeger noted. “It’s important to test the generalizability of our results. We find that first impressions are largely based on how much a person’s facial features resemble a smile or a frown, but is that also true for people in China, Chile, or Chad?”

The study, “Which Facial Features Are Central in Impression Formation?”, was authored by Bastian Jaeger and Alex L. Jones.

RELATED

His psychosis was a mystery—until doctors learned about ChatGPT’s health advice
Psychopathy

Female killers in Sweden show low psychopathy, primarily reactive motives

August 13, 2025

A nationwide Swedish study finds most women who commit lethal violence act in emotionally charged situations, with low psychopathy scores and little planning. Severe mental disorders were linked to a more complex blend of reactive and instrumental features.

Read moreDetails
Lonely individuals tend to view themselves as a burden to others
Sexism

Women face backlash when expressing anger about gender inequality

August 12, 2025

A new study suggests that women who express anger about gender inequality tend to be judged less warmly and receive less public support, but framing their anger as concern for others can partially reduce this backlash.

Read moreDetails
Lonely individuals tend to view themselves as a burden to others
Mental Health

Lonely individuals tend to view themselves as a burden to others

August 12, 2025

A study of U.S. adults finds that loneliness is linked to feeling like a burden in close relationships. The research also highlights how internal physiological states—such as heart rate variability—may shape how individuals cope with loneliness and self-doubt.

Read moreDetails
Neuroscientists identify a reversible biological mechanism behind drug-induced cognitive deficits
Cognitive Science

Dopamine’s role in learning may be broader than previously thought

August 11, 2025

New research reveals dopamine helps the brain juggle fast, flexible problem-solving with gradual habit formation. By boosting working memory use and enhancing trial-and-error learning, dopamine influences both how quickly we learn and how costly mental effort feels.

Read moreDetails
Assimilation-induced dehumanization: Psychology research uncovers a dark side effect of AI
Social Psychology

What really keeps people committed to long-term goals? This study offers a key insight

August 11, 2025

Why do so many people fail to achieve the goals they set for themselves? New research spanning multiple cultures and contexts offers a surprising explanation—and points to a way forward that most people never consider.

Read moreDetails
People with narcissistic tendencies report more ostracism and are more often excluded
Political Psychology

Intellectual humility is linked to less political and religious polarization across the board

August 10, 2025

A large online study indicates that intellectual humility is linked to less hostility toward political and religious opponents. The effect was seen across political parties and belief systems, and persisted even after controlling for the strength of participants’ convictions.

Read moreDetails
Exercise can reduce feelings of hopelessness among patients in suicide crisis, pilot study finds
Cognitive Science

Physically active individuals tend to have slightly better cognitive abilities on average

August 10, 2025

A new meta-analysis suggests that while physical activity generally has a small positive impact on cognition, outdoor moderate-to-vigorous exercise paired with cognitively challenging activities delivers the biggest gains across age groups, from preschoolers to older adults.

Read moreDetails
People with narcissistic tendencies report more ostracism and are more often excluded
Anxiety

Sleep may amplify negative memory bias in anxious youth

August 10, 2025

Researchers have found that children and young adolescents with higher anxiety tend to generalize negative memories more after sleep, raising questions about how nighttime memory processes could reinforce anxiety-related thought patterns during a sensitive developmental stage.

Read moreDetails

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Long-term study finds widening gap in social isolation between sexual minority and heterosexual adults

How parents talk about uncertainty may shape children’s intellectual humility

Teen sleep habits may shape brain connectivity linked to behavior problems

Traditional beliefs can shift the link between beauty and women’s sexual openness, new research suggests

Parents’ attachment style linked to how deeply they connect with positive memories

Female killers in Sweden show low psychopathy, primarily reactive motives

Depressed individuals have heightened C-reactive protein-to-albumin ratio

Genetic link found between suicide risk and brain structure in large-scale study

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy