Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

Masks hinder our ability to recognize facial expressions of sadness and fear

by Eric W. Dolan
March 25, 2023
in Cognitive Science
(Image by Наркологическая Клиника from Pixabay)

(Image by Наркологическая Клиника from Pixabay)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay informed on the latest psychology and neuroscience research—follow PsyPost on LinkedIn for daily updates and insights.

Face masks can make it harder to recognize facial expressions that stimulate empathy, such as sadness, according to new research published in the scientific journal Psychophysiology.

Previous research has indicated that masks can impair the recognition of basic facial expressions. But little is known about neurophysiological implications of this phenomenon. The authors of the new study set out to investigate how face masking could affect different stages of neural processing involved in facial expression comprehension.

“After a 2-year pandemic era, necessarily characterized by a pervasive and worldwide use of surgical masks, we studied in the laboratory the neural and behavioral responses of observers exposed to faces wearing or not wearing surgical masks. We wished to understand the specific effects of facial expression deprivation on the human mind and brain,” explained study author Alice Mado Proverbio, a professor of cognitive neuroscience at the University of Milano-Bicocca and head of the Cognitive Electrophysiology Lab.

The study involved 26 right-handed university students (13 women and 13 men) with an average age of 22.35 years. They participated in a facial expression categorization task involving six emotions while EEG was continuously recorded. The task used pictures of 5 female and 5 male actors/actresses displaying 6 spontaneous emotional expressions in masked and unmasked conditions. Emotional prime words were presented before the faces to create congruent or incongruent conditions.

The researchers found that response times were slower in the incongruent condition compared to the congruent condition, indicating an emotional priming effect. In other words, participants were faster to identify a sad-looking facial expression when the prime was “SAD” compared to when the prime was “HAPPY.”

But masks appeared to have a detrimental impact on emotion recognition. This was particularly true for disgust, sadness, and fear, which were not recognized more quickly when preceded by congruent primes.

When faces were shown with masks, a specific brain response called N170 became larger compared to when the faces were shown normally (unmasked). This suggests that the brain had to work harder or for a longer time to process the limited information in the masked faces. The N170 response is associated with the “fusiform face area,” which is involved in encoding facial features and affected by various factors such as familiarity and expression.

“It was found not only that face masking reduced the ability to read facial expressions but also that it polarized the spectrum of emotional signals conveyed to others toward the negative/positive dimension of happiness/anger,” Proverbio told PsyPost. “It seems that face masking specifically impairs the communication of softer emotions such as sadness, fear or disgust, which usually trigger an empathic resonance in the observer. The limited recognition of people’s distress might lead to a reduction of personal concern and empathic response in the observers.”

Interestingly, masks did not exhibit any negative effects on the accuracy or response times to angry faces. “We were surprised to find out that mask covering did not impair at all the recognition of angry faces, which may increase even more the likelihood of getting angry in social interactions,” Proverbio remarked.

The findings are in line with previous research, which has found that masks make it harder for people to interpret facial expressions and might even change the way faces are cognitively processed.

“It must be understood whether this deficit in social cognition can have direct consequences on people’s aggressiveness, their inclination to help, cooperate or forgive others,” Proverbio said. “Indeed, the lack of facial cues about the others’ suffering might impair the ability to understand people’s emotions, and to experience feelings of tenderness, pity or sympathy.”

“It also remains to be clarified whether not being able to perceive pitying, empathic, understanding or loving facial expressions (but simply happy or angry ones) would increase stress levels in individuals seeking psychological comfort,” the researcher added. “Further studies will also have to clarify whether perceiving masked human faces for continued and enduring periods would be able to affect the development of children’s empathic capacity.”

The study, “Facemasks selectively impair the recognition of facial expressions that stimulate empathy: An ERP study“, was authored by Alice Mado Proverbio, Alice Cerri, and Cristina Gallotta.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Neuroimaging study suggests mindfulness meditation lowers sensory gating
Cognitive Science

Neuroimaging study suggests mindfulness meditation lowers sensory gating

June 7, 2025

A new study finds that mindfulness meditators are more likely to report feeling a touch — even when none occurs — and that this sensitivity is linked to altered brain rhythms.

Read moreDetails
Your brain’s insulation might become emergency energy during a marathon
Cognitive Science

Scientists map the hidden architecture of the brain’s default mode network

June 5, 2025

A new study reveals that the brain’s default mode network is made up of distinct anatomical types that support both internal thoughts and external processing. This structural diversity helps explain the network’s role in everything from memory to imagination.

Read moreDetails
Sleep deprivation reduces attention and cognitive processing capacity
Cognitive Science

Sleep deprivation reduces attention and cognitive processing capacity

May 31, 2025

A new study shows that 36 hours without sleep impairs table tennis players’ reaction times, attention, and brain connectivity. The findings reveal how acute sleep deprivation disrupts spatial cognitive processing, with potential consequences for athletic performance and decision-making under pressure.

Read moreDetails
Neuroscientists pinpoint part of the brain that deciphers memory from new experience
Memory

Neuroscientists find individual differences in memory response to amygdala stimulation

May 31, 2025

Stimulating the brain’s amygdala during memory formation can boost recall after 24 hours, a new study finds. But the effect varies: some people’s memory improves, others’ worsens—and baseline memory performance appears to be the best predictor of outcome.

Read moreDetails
MDMA therapy: Side effects appear mild, but there are problems with the evidence
Cognitive Science

Consciousness remains a mystery after major theory showdown

May 30, 2025

A groundbreaking collaboration has tested two of the most influential theories of consciousness—global neuronal workspace and integrated information theory. While neither came out on top, the project marks a major shift in how scientists approach one of the mind’s biggest mysteries.

Read moreDetails
A common calorie-free sweetener alters brain activity and appetite control, new research suggests
Cognitive Science

A common calorie-free sweetener alters brain activity and appetite control, new research suggests

May 30, 2025

A recent brain imaging study finds that sucralose, unlike sugar, increases activity in the hypothalamus and boosts hunger, suggesting that calorie-free sweetness may confuse the brain’s appetite control system.

Read moreDetails
Delusion-like cognitive biases predict conspiracy theory belief
Cognitive Science

Delusion-like cognitive biases predict conspiracy theory belief

May 28, 2025

People prone to conspiracy theories may share cognitive tendencies with those who experience delusional thinking. Two new studies suggest that biases like anomalous perception and impulsive reasoning help explain why some are more likely to embrace conspiratorial beliefs.

Read moreDetails
Psychology study sheds light on why some moments seem to fly by
Memory

Psychology study sheds light on why some moments seem to fly by

May 24, 2025

A new study suggests life feels like it speeds up during periods of personal growth and satisfaction. Rather than routine making time seem short, researchers found that fulfilled, nostalgic memories are more likely to make the past feel like a blur.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Socioeconomic background tied to distinct brain and behavioral patterns

Do dark personality traits predict vote choices in U.S. presidential elections?

Anxious-depressed individuals underestimate themselves even when they’re right

Ayahuasca entity encounters linked to lasting religious belief changes, especially in men

Older women dating younger men report higher sexual satisfaction, study finds

Genetic and biological clues point to inflammation’s role in mental health

Study links anorexia nervosa to elevated opioid receptor levels in brain’s reward centers

Feeling unseen fuels support for the populist right, study finds

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy