Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology Political Psychology

More biased = more trustworthy? New research uncovers a troubling trend among Democrats and Republicans

by Eric W. Dolan
February 6, 2025
in Political Psychology
(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

(Photo credit: Adobe Stock)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Follow PsyPost on Google News

A new study reveals how the words we use to describe political events can impact how trustworthy we perceive the speaker to be, and even shape our own opinions about the event itself. The research, published in the journal Cognition, shows that people tend to trust speakers who use language that aligns with their own political views. However, this same partisan language can damage trust and increase disagreement when it’s heard by those with opposing political beliefs.

Political polarization is a growing problem, not just in the United States, but globally. Researchers wanted to understand how the language used to describe political events might contribute to this increasing divide. We’re seeing more and more partisan language used by politicians, news organizations, and even everyday people on social media.

For instance, when describing the events at the U.S. Capitol on January 6th, many Republicans use the word “protest,” while many Democrats prefer terms like “insurrection” or “riot.” The new study aimed to isolate the impact of these specific word choices, examining how they affect both our perceptions of the speaker and our own understanding of what happened. The researchers hypothesized that individuals may be using this type of language more often to appear in a favorable light to people who already agree with them.

“Data shows that people increasingly harbour negative feelings towards their political opponents. A primary goal of this research is to assess how aspects of the current political climate—such as the prevalence of politically-biased language—contribute to deepening political divides,” said study author Alexander Walker, a Banting Postdoctoral Fellow at Brown University.

The research involved two experiments with a total of 1,121 participants from the United States, all of whom identified as either Democrats or Republicans. These participants were recruited online through services like Amazon Mechanical Turk and Prolific.

In the first experiment, participants were presented with descriptions of various politically charged events. Each event had three different descriptions: one using language favored by liberals (e.g., “expand voting rights”), one using language favored by conservatives (e.g., “reduce election security”), and one using neutral language (e.g., “expand mail-in voting”). These descriptions were presented as public statements made by fictional people. Alongside each statement, participants were also given a detailed, factual account of the event, which they were told to consider completely accurate. This was done to ensure everyone had a common understanding of the underlying facts, regardless of the partisan language used.

Participants were then asked to rate the speaker on several measures. They judged how trustworthy and moral the speaker seemed, how open-minded they appeared, and how much criticism they deserved. Participants also indicated how interested they would be in having a political discussion with the speaker, and what political party they believed the speaker belonged to.

The second experiment focused on how partisan language influences people’s own opinions about the events. Participants were presented with the same events, again described using either liberal-leaning, conservative-leaning, or neutral language. However, in this experiment, some participants received additional, detailed information about the event, while others received only the brief, partisan or neutral description. The key measure in this experiment was how much participants agreed or disagreed with the actions described in the event.

The results of the first experiment showed that people respond very differently to partisan language depending on their own political views. When speakers used language that matched the participant’s political leaning (in-group language), they were seen as more trustworthy, moral, and open-minded. Participants were also more willing to engage in a political discussion with these speakers.

However, the opposite was true when speakers used language associated with the opposing political party (out-group language). These speakers were viewed as less trustworthy, less moral, more closed-minded, and more deserving of criticism. Participants were also much less interested in talking politics with them. The more strongly a participant perceived a speaker as belonging to their own political party, the more positively they rated them.

“People view speakers describing political events in a way that aligns with their ideological leanings as highly trustworthy,” Walker told PsyPost. “Thus, as audiences become more politically polarized, people may increasingly view politically-biased news as more trustworthy than that which aims to be more neutral.”

“While describing events using politically-biased language was the best way to garner trust from political in-group members, describing these events using more neutral language was the best way to garner trust across political lines (i.e., from both Democrats and Republicans alike). Thus, despite the political leanings of our participants, they still both agreed that the presented politically neutral statements accurately described a set of contentious political events.”

The second experiment revealed that partisan language not only affects our view of the speaker, but also polarizes our own attitudes about the event itself. When events were described using language aligned with a participant’s political views, their opinions about the event became more extreme in that direction.

This effect was particularly strong when participants didn’t have much additional information about the event. When more details were provided, the polarizing effect of the partisan language was reduced, though it didn’t disappear completely. This finding implies that when people know more about the details of an event, they are less likely to let a single word or phrase dictate their overall opinion. Even so, the tendency to interpret events in a way that reinforces preexisting political beliefs remained evident.

“We find evidence suggesting that people are incentivized to describe political events in a manner that aligns with the political biases of their audience,” Walker said. “We also show that exposure to politically-biased descriptions of political events a) promotes negative evaluations of political out-group members and b) shapes how people view political events.”

But as with all research, there are some caveats to consider. The scenarios used in both experiments, while carefully designed, represented only a subset of the many possible events and linguistic choices that occur in everyday political discourse. The researchers note that large-scale studies using real-world data from social media, political speeches, or news reports could help confirm and expand on these findings.

“One limitation of the current work is that, unlike in real-world contexts, participants in our study did not have information about the people describing political events,” Walker noted. “This allowed us to isolate the impact of politically-biased language on peoples’ political attitudes and judgments of trustworthiness (e.g., trust in speakers describing a political event). However, it is an open question whether knowledge of a person’s or organization’s political leanings limits the persuasive influence of partisan rhetoric.”

The study highlights an important societal challenge: even when information is objectively accurate, the way it is framed can have powerful effects on public opinion and interpersonal trust. While much attention is often focused on the dangers of misinformation, this research demonstrates that even truthful language can contribute to polarization if it is presented in a way that appeals to partisan identities.

“Much attention has been paid to the negative impact of misinformation,” Walker said. “However, we show that information need not be objectively false to facilitate distrust of political opponents or polarize opinion across party lines.”

“A long-term goal of this work is to better understand growing partisan divides by understanding the interplay between peoples’ political environment and political attitudes. For example, does politically-biased (yet not objectively false) news make people more extreme in their ideological attitudes? Do these more extreme attitudes result in people viewing politically-biased information as most trustworthy? If so, how can this divisive cycle be interrupted?”

The study, “Partisan language in a polarized world: In-group language provides reputational benefits to speakers while polarizing audiences,” was authored by Alexander C. Walker, Jonathan A. Fugelsang, and Derek J. Koehler.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

New psychology study sheds light on mysterious “feelings of presence” during isolation
Political Psychology

People who think “everyone agrees with me” are more likely to support populism

July 1, 2025

People who wrongly believe that most others share their political views are more likely to support populist ideas, according to a new study. These false beliefs can erode trust in democratic institutions and fuel resentment toward political elites.

Read moreDetails
Radical leaders inspire stronger devotion because they make followers feel significant, study finds
Political Psychology

Radical leaders inspire stronger devotion because they make followers feel significant, study finds

June 28, 2025

A new study finds that voters are more motivated by radical political leaders than moderates, because supporting bold causes makes them feel personally significant—driving greater activism, sacrifice, and long-term engagement across elections in the United States and Poland.

Read moreDetails
Political ambivalence has a surprising relationship with support for violence
Authoritarianism

New study sheds light on the psychological roots of collective violence

June 21, 2025

A new study from Lebanon finds that people with authoritarian beliefs tend to oppose violence against political leaders, while those high in social dominance orientation are more likely to support violence against rival group members.

Read moreDetails
Epistemic mistrust and dogmatism predict preference for authoritarian-looking leaders
Authoritarianism

Epistemic mistrust and dogmatism predict preference for authoritarian-looking leaders

June 20, 2025

A new study suggests that the way people learn to trust others early in life can shape their political ideology and preference for strong, dominant leaders—though not directly, but through dogmatic thinking and broader political attitudes.

Read moreDetails
Individual traits, not environment, predict gun violence among gun-carrying youth
Political Psychology

Republican women and Democratic men often break with party lines on gun policy

June 19, 2025

New research shows that Americans’ views on gun policy are shaped by the intersection of gender and partisanship, with Republican women and Democratic men often expressing positions that differ from those typically associated with their party.

Read moreDetails
Troubling study shows “politics can trump truth” to a surprising degree, regardless of education or analytical ability
Donald Trump

Racial insecurity helped shield Trump from Republican backlash after Capitol riot, study suggests

June 18, 2025

Despite widespread condemnation of the January 6th attack, many white Republicans remained loyal to Trump—especially those who perceived anti-white discrimination. A new study shows how racial status threat can protect political leaders from the consequences of norm violations.

Read moreDetails
Poor sleep may shrink brain regions vulnerable to Alzheimer’s disease, study suggests
Political Psychology

Christian nationalists tend to imagine God as benevolent, angry over sins, and engaged

June 14, 2025

Christians who believe God is benevolent, engaged, and angered by sin are more likely to support religious nationalism, according to a new study. This worldview was also associated with conspiracy mentality and xenophobic attitudes.

Read moreDetails
Fear predicts authoritarian attitudes across cultures, with conservatives most affected
Authoritarianism

Fear predicts authoritarian attitudes across cultures, with conservatives most affected

June 13, 2025

A sweeping study of over 84,000 people across 59 countries found that individuals who feel threatened by crime, poverty, or instability are more likely to support authoritarian governance—especially in Western nations and among politically right-leaning individuals.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Frequent dreams and nightmares surged worldwide during the COVID-19 pandemic

Vagus nerve signals influence food intake more in higher socio-economic groups

People who think “everyone agrees with me” are more likely to support populism

What is the most attractive body fat percentage for men? New research offers an answer

Longer antidepressant use linked to more severe, long-lasting withdrawal symptoms, study finds

New psychology study sheds light on mysterious “feelings of presence” during isolation

New study reveals how MDMA rewires serotonin and oxytocin systems in the brain

Ghosting and ‘breadcrumbing’: the psychological impact of our bad behaviour on dating apps

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy