Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Cognitive Science

New insights into how anxiety and curiosity affect mind wandering tendencies

by Vladimir Hedrih
November 12, 2023
in Cognitive Science, Social Psychology
(Photo credit: OpenAI's DALL·E)

(Photo credit: OpenAI's DALL·E)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay informed on the latest psychology and neuroscience research—follow PsyPost on LinkedIn for daily updates and insights.

New research has found that curious individuals tend to be more prone to both intentional and unintentional mind wandering. On the other hand, anxious individuals tend to have poorer executive control. Individuals with poorer executive control, in turn, tend to be less able to suppress tendencies of the mind to wander, leading to higher unintentional mind wandering tendencies. The study was published in Personality and Individual Differences.

Mind wandering, also known as daydreaming, refers to the phenomenon in which a person’s attention shifts away from their immediate task or external environment and becomes focused on unrelated thoughts, memories, or fantasies. It often occurs involuntarily and can include a wide range of mental experiences, from planning for the future to reminiscing about the past.

Mind wandering is a common and normal aspect of human cognition, but excessive or uncontrolled mind wandering can sometimes be distracting and affect productivity. It has attracted a lot of research attention in the past decades and researchers proposed various psychological mechanisms in an attempt to explain it. The tendency to daydream has been linked to anxiety and depression, but also to creative thinking. Studies have shown that executive control capacities of an individual also play a role in this phenomenon.

The author of the study, Takahiro Sekiguchi of Tokyo Gakugei University, aimed to investigate the influence of curiosity and anxiety on mind wandering. His focus was primarily on epistemic curiosity, which is the desire to gain new knowledge and insights about the world, motivated by a genuine interest in learning and discovery. Epistemic curiosity can manifest in two forms: diversive curiosity, which entails an interest in a broad spectrum of subjects and topics, and specific curiosity, which is more concentrated on particular areas of interest. To explore these aspects, Sekiguchi conducted two separate studies.

Participants of the first study were 260 psychology students recruited from the Tokyo Gakugei University in Japan. They completed assessments of epistemic curiosity (the Epistemic Curiosity Scale), mind wandering tendency (the Mind Wandering questionnaire), trait anxiety (the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory), and executive control (the Effortful Control Scale for Adults). Executive control refers to cognitive processes responsible for managing and regulating higher-order functions like planning, decision-making, attention, problem-solving, and self-control. It enables individuals to adapt to changing circumstances and achieve their goals.

The second study was an online survey. Participants were 328 native Japanese speakers recruited through a crowdsourcing service CrowdWorks. Their average age was 30 years. These individuals completed assessments of epistemic curiosity, trait anxiety, and executive control using the same assessments as in the first study.

The findings from the first study revealed no significant correlation between either form of curiosity and the propensity for mind wandering. Nonetheless, it was observed that individuals with higher anxiety levels were more susceptible to mind wandering and exhibited weaker executive control capabilities.

Sekiguchi developed and tested a statistical model suggesting that the relationship between anxiety and mind wandering is mediated by executive control. This model posits that increased anxiety results in diminished executive control, which in turn leads to a greater likelihood of mind wandering. The study’s results supported this model, indicating that the connection between trait anxiety and mind wandering is not direct but occurs through the impact of anxiety on executive control.

The second study revealed only a faint link between diversive curiosity and unintentional mind wandering. On average, individuals with a broad range of interests were marginally more inclined to experience mind wandering than those with more focused interests.

When examining the interplay of these factors through a statistical model, it was suggested that diversive curiosity, as opposed to specific curiosity, could potentially increase the likelihood of both intentional and unintentional mind wandering. Additionally, the study reaffirmed that anxiety impairs executive control, which subsequently increases the propensity for unintentional mind wandering.

The study author concluded, “This research demonstrates that trait anxiety is indirectly linked to the tendency for mind wandering through executive control. In contrast, epistemic curiosity, a more positive and socially valuable personality trait, has a direct influence on the propensity for mind wandering. These insights suggest that mind wandering is a phenomenon rooted in socially desirable qualities, lending a positive aspect to this construct.”

The study sheds light on the psychological mechanisms underpinning mind wandering. However, it also has limitations that need to be taken into account. Notably, the study design does not allow any cause-and-effect conclusions to be drawn from the data. Additionally, the link between diversive curiosity and unintentional mind wandering is very weak.

The study, “Curiosity makes your mind wander: Effects of epistemic curiosity and trait anxiety on mind wandering”, was authored by Takahiro Sekiguchi.

TweetSendScanShareSendPin3ShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Scientists reveal ChatGPT’s left-wing bias — and how to “jailbreak” it
Artificial Intelligence

ChatGPT and “cognitive debt”: New study suggests AI might be hurting your brain’s ability to think

July 1, 2025

Researchers at MIT investigated how writing with ChatGPT affects brain activity and recall. Their findings indicate that reliance on AI may lead to reduced mental engagement, prompting concerns about cognitive “offloading” and its implications for education.

Read moreDetails
New psychology study sheds light on mysterious “feelings of presence” during isolation
Cognitive Science

Vagus nerve signals influence food intake more in higher socio-economic groups

July 1, 2025

Researchers have found that internal physiological cues—like signals from the vagus nerve—play a stronger role in guiding eating behavior among wealthier individuals, offering new insight into why socio-economic status is linked to differences in diet and health.

Read moreDetails
New psychology study sheds light on mysterious “feelings of presence” during isolation
Political Psychology

People who think “everyone agrees with me” are more likely to support populism

July 1, 2025

People who wrongly believe that most others share their political views are more likely to support populist ideas, according to a new study. These false beliefs can erode trust in democratic institutions and fuel resentment toward political elites.

Read moreDetails
Researchers identify neural mechanism behind memory prioritization
Social Psychology

People who are more likely to die seem to care less about the future

June 30, 2025

Individuals with higher mortality risk—as judged by actuarial life insurance data—tend to care less about long-term consequences. They show more impulsivity and less future planning, consistent with evolutionary theories that link time horizon to environmental and internal health cues.

Read moreDetails
Researchers identify neural mechanism behind memory prioritization
Memory

Researchers identify neural mechanism behind memory prioritization

June 30, 2025

A new brain imaging study shows that when people try to remember multiple things, their brains give more precise attention to the most important item. The frontal cortex helps allocate memory resources, boosting accuracy for high-priority information.

Read moreDetails
Scientists show how you’re unknowingly sealing yourself in an information bubble
Cognitive Science

Scientists show how you’re unknowingly sealing yourself in an information bubble

June 29, 2025

Scientists have found that belief polarization doesn’t always come from misinformation or social media bubbles. Instead, it often begins with a simple search. Our choice of words—and the algorithm’s response—can subtly seal us inside our own informational comfort zones.

Read moreDetails
Muscle contractions release chemical signals that promote brain network development
Memory

Sleep helps stitch memories into cognitive maps, according to new neuroscience breakthrough

June 28, 2025

Scientists have discovered that forming a mental map of a new environment takes more than just recognizing individual places—it also requires sleep. The study highlights how weakly tuned neurons gradually become synchronized to encode space as a connected whole.

Read moreDetails
Radical leaders inspire stronger devotion because they make followers feel significant, study finds
Political Psychology

Radical leaders inspire stronger devotion because they make followers feel significant, study finds

June 28, 2025

A new study finds that voters are more motivated by radical political leaders than moderates, because supporting bold causes makes them feel personally significant—driving greater activism, sacrifice, and long-term engagement across elections in the United States and Poland.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Could creatine slow cognitive decline? Mouse study reveals promising effects on brain aging

ChatGPT and “cognitive debt”: New study suggests AI might be hurting your brain’s ability to think

Frequent dreams and nightmares surged worldwide during the COVID-19 pandemic

Vagus nerve signals influence food intake more in higher socio-economic groups

People who think “everyone agrees with me” are more likely to support populism

What is the most attractive body fat percentage for men? New research offers an answer

Longer antidepressant use linked to more severe, long-lasting withdrawal symptoms, study finds

New psychology study sheds light on mysterious “feelings of presence” during isolation

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy